Abstract

Our study analyzes the fertility effects of the 1994 genocide in Rwanda. We study the effects of violence on both the duration time to the first birth in the early post-genocide period and on the total number of post-genocide births per woman up to 15 years following the conflict. We use individual-level data from Demographic and Health Surveys, estimating survival and count data models. This article contributes to the literature on the demographic effects of violent conflict by testing two channels through which conflict influences fertility: (1) the type of violence exposure as measured by the death of a child or sibling, and (2) the conflict-induced change in local demographic conditions as captured by the change in the district-level sex ratio. Results indicate the genocide had heterogeneous effects on fertility, depending on the type of violence experienced by the woman, her age cohort, parity, and the time horizon (5, 10, and 15 years after the genocide). There is strong evidence of a child replacement effect. Having experienced the death of a child during the genocide increases both the hazard of having a child in the five years following the genocide and the total number of post-genocide births. Experiencing sibling death during the genocide significantly lowers post-genocide fertility in both the short-run and the long-run. Finally, a reduction in the local sex ratio negatively impacts the hazard of having a child in the five years following the genocide, especially for older women.

Introduction

Does violent conflict affect fertility? Various studies have addressed this important issue, finding that violent conflict influences fertility during and after a conflict. Effects are shown to vary across empirical contexts (e.g., Agadjanian and Prata 2002; Heuveline and Poch 2007; Lindstrom and Berhanu 1999; Woldemicael 2008). Yet, little evidence is available on the mechanisms through which violent events may affect individual fertility, possibly explaining the differences in the direction of the effect identified in previous studies.

We use individual-level data to provide empirical evidence on various mechanisms linking conflict to fertility. We focus on the effects of the 1994 genocide in Rwanda, one of the most devastating violent conflicts since World War II, during which at least 500,000 individuals died within just 100 days. The Rwandan genocide provides a suitable setting for empirically exploring this question for two reasons. First, data on the fertility histories of Rwandan women are available from multiple post-genocide surveys that are representative at the national level and of high quality, which is rare for conflict-affected countries. Second, the fact that the Rwandan genocide was extremely violent and of very short duration reduces the possibility that other events may confound the causal identification of the fertility effects of the conflict.

In our analysis, we use survival and count data models to study the effect of violent conflict on the duration time to the first birth after genocide (i.e., the hazard of having a child in the first five years following the genocide) and on the total number of post-genocide births per woman up to 15 years after the conflict. We focus on two main channels through which conflict may affect fertility. First, we study the effect of different types of individual exposure to violence. In particular, we consider the effect of experiencing the death of a child or sibling during the genocide on a woman’s fertility outcomes. Second, we consider the role of local demographic conditions. We focus on the genocide-induced change in the commune-level1 sex ratio—with a severe imbalance of men to women in the post-conflict period—and test its effects on fertility outcomes.

Our main source of data consists of three cross-sectional waves of Demographic and Health Surveys (DHS) collected in Rwanda in 2000, 2005, and 2010. This span allows us to disaggregate the effects of the genocide on fertility over time, distinguishing among the short- (1995–2000), medium- (2000–2005), and long-term (2005–2010) post-genocide periods. Thereby, we provide a comprehensive perspective on the conflict-induced adjustments in fertility.

We present three main findings. First, we find strong evidence of a child replacement effect. Experiencing the death of a child during the genocide increases both the hazard of having a child in the five years following the genocide and the total number of births in the post-genocide period. Second, experiencing the death of a sibling during the genocide significantly lowers post-genocide fertility. The effect is the strongest if a woman loses a younger sibling, which suggests a psychological mechanism. Finally, the genocide-induced reduction in the local sex ratio has a negative impact on the hazard of having a child in the five years following the genocide. The effect is particularly strong for older women.

Literature

Literature on the Effects of Conflict on Fertility

Traditionally, researchers have looked at the impact of violent conflict on fertility using aggregate measures of fertility as the outcome variable. Most studies have found a decline in fertility during conflict, followed by an increase in the early postwar period, as well as a gradual decline in fertility in the longer term for most conflicts (Hill 2004). Yet, evidence is mixed. For instance, Iqbal (2010), examining cross-country data, found no significant effects of war on aggregate fertility. Urdal and Che (2013), using time-series cross-country data for the 1970–2005 period, showed that armed conflicts are associated with higher overall fertility in low-income countries.

More recent studies investigated the impact of violent conflict on fertility outcomes at the micro level. Some studies found that conflict tends to increase fertility in, for example, Cambodia (Islam et al. 2016), the Occupied Palestinian Territories (Khawaja 2000), and Tajikistan (Shemyakina 2011). In the context of Rwanda et al. (2005) showed that female refugees have higher fertility rates than their nonrefugee counterparts, but Rogall and Yanagizawa-Drott (2014) found an increase in post-genocide fertility only among young women.

In contrast, evidence also supports that exposure to conflict or periods of political instability may result in a decline in fertility.2 Studies have found this for Bangladesh (Curlin et al. 1976), Kazakhstan (Agadjanian et al. 2008), Angola (Agadjanian and Prata 2002), Cambodia (Heuveline and Poch 2007), Eritrea (Woldemicael 2008), Ethiopia (Lindstrom and Berhanu 1999), the Occupied Palestinian Territories (Khawaja et al. 2009), and Tajikistan (Clifford et al. 2010). Interestingly, several of these studies also found a rebound of fertility after the crisis ends (e.g., Agadjanian and Prata 2002; Heuveline anf Poch 2007; Lindstrom and Berhanu 1999).

Literature on the Genocide in Rwanda

The Rwandan genocide is one of the most violent conflicts in the history of humanity. The genocide broke out on April 6, 1994, after the plane of President Habyarimana was shot down while approaching Kigali airport, killing all passengers.3 An extremist Hutu militia known as Interahamwe, the Rwandan Armed Forces (FAR), and Rwandan police forces organized massacres against the Tutsi minority and, to a lesser degree, moderate Hutu intellectuals who were opposed to the regime. Death toll estimates range between 500,000 to more than 1 million—about 10 % of the 1994 population (Desforges 1999; Verpoorten 2005). Most of these individuals were Tutsi who were killed in one-sided violence, resulting in the death of an estimated 75 % of the Tutsi population (Desforges 1999). A smaller number of soldiers died in combat between the FAR and the Rwandan Patriotic Front (RPF), a rebel army of exiled Tutsi invading Rwanda from Uganda. The RPF eventually stopped the genocide in July 1994 and took power.

The Rwandan genocide is well studied, with a very large literature on both its determinants and consequences (e.g., Akresh and de Walque 2008; Akresh et al. 2011; André and Platteau 1998; de Walque and Verwimp 2010; Justino and Verwimp 2013; La Mattina 2017; Lopez and Wodon 2005; Schindler and Verpoorten 2013; Verpoorten 2009, 2012; Yanagizawa-Drott 2014). Results show that the genocide severely affected household income, poverty, education outcomes, health, and the incidence of domestic violence. The genocide also had a large impact on factors affecting demographic dynamics and fertility, such as sexual behavior (Elveborg Lindskog 2016) and refugee status (Verwimp and Van Bavel 2005). Rogall and Yanagizawa-Drott (2014) found evidence of a positive effect of the reception of radio waves (their proxy for exposure to violence) on total fertility for their young cohort, but they did not find significant effects for their two older cohorts. Yet, they focused only on the effect of violence on total fertility and did not explore the mechanisms explaining these effects at the micro level.

Conceptual Framework

The economic literature suggests that conflict may affect fertility through different demand and supply channels (Brück and Schindler 2009; Verwimp et al. 2009; Williams et al. 2012). On the one hand, conflict-induced deterioration in the economic conditions may reduce fertility because couples respond to a sudden decline in income by delaying marriage and birth in order to smooth consumption (Lee 1990; Rindfuss et al. 1978). On the other hand, conflict may increase the demand for children when parents suffer the loss of a child (Agadjanian and Prata 2002). Because children are a potential source of economic support for parents in old age (Caldwell et al. 1986), conflict may increase fertility because the value of the insurance role of children increases under conditions of economic insecurity (Cain 1983; Nugent 1985). Finally, conflict may affect the demand for children by decreasing a woman’s education attainment, thereby encouraging early female marriage (La Mattina 2017).

On the supply side, conflict may affect fertility because it can affect the local sex ratio and the marriage market (Buvinic et al. 2013). In particular, large numbers of young men being mobilized for warfare would lead to both delayed marriages and a decline in marital fertility (Urdal and Che 2013). Moreover, conflict may influence fertility through its impact on reproductive health services (Verwimp and Van Bavel 2005).

The psychology literature suggests several additional possible mechanisms linking conflict and fertility. Exposure to a tragic event, such as the death of a family member, may affect fertility because it reduces the desire for children, decreases coital frequency, and negatively affects the women’s physiological capacity to carry a child to term, thereby leading to a higher incidence of miscarriages (Frankenberg et al. 2015; Nobles et al. 2015; Norris et al. 2002). Moreover, psychological stress and the decline in nutritional status associated with conflict may reduce fecundity and coital frequency (Kidane 1989). On the contrary, terror management theory suggests that people react to the exposure to violent events by adhering more closely to traditional values, such as by focusing on their household and having more children (Rodgers et al. 2005; Vail et al. 2012).

Finally, sociological research emphasizes that fertility could also be influenced by variation in conflict-induced mortality at the level of community or ethnic group (Sandberg 2006). In particular, the positive association between mortality and fertility is expected to be stronger when a specific group is targeted or a large share of the population dies as a consequence of the conflict (Heuveline and Poch 2007).

The aforementioned heterogeneous empirical evidence and the various possible mechanisms linking conflict to fertility that we describe here suggest that both the effects and mechanisms are likely to vary with the specific conflict. In particular, the literature suggests that what matters is the type and duration of the conflict, the type of violence experienced by the population, and the induced changes in the local economic and social conditions (including the local sex ratio) (Nobles et al. 2015; Urdal and Che 2013; Verwimp et al. 2017). In our analysis, we capture the specific characteristics of the Rwandan genocide by focusing on two channels: (1) the type and intensity of individual exposure to violence, as measured by either child mortality or a woman’s sibling’s death; and (2) the conflict-induced change in local demographic conditions, as measured by the commune-level sex ratio. Our theoretical predictions regarding the expected impact of those forms of exposure to the genocide on fertility guide our empirical analysis.

Type and Intensity of Individual Exposure to Violence

Child Mortality

In general, household demand theory has no clear prediction as to the effect of child mortality on fertility (Schultz 1997). The target fertility model provides the intuitive basis for the mechanisms that predict a positive correlation between child mortality and fertility. The literature focuses on two main mechanisms leading to a positive correlation between child mortality and fertility: replacement (child replacement hypothesis)4 and insurance (child survival hypothesis) (Bousmah 2017; Hossain et al. 2007; Montgomery and Cohen 1998; Nobles et al. 2015; Pörtner 2001; Preston 1978; Schultz 1969; Wolpin 1997). Instead, price theory yields ambiguous predictions regarding fertility. The basic model indicates that parents respond to child mortality by increasing the number of births they demand (Ben-Porath 1976; Sah 1991). In particular, the positive effect of child mortality on subsequent fertility is reinforced by reduced expected returns on investments in child education, which induces a substitution of quantity for quality of children (Kalemli-Ozcan 2003). Yet, when the fact that children are costly is considered in the optimization problem, the optimal response to higher mortality varies with the properties of the utility function (Ben-Porath 1976). In this more general setting, the sign of the effect of a child’s death depends on the relative strength of the replacement motive (which tends to increase fertility) and the income effect (which tends to reduce fertility).

The psychology literature suggests other possible explanations for the link between child death and fertility. Child death may reduce fertility because it reduces the desire for children and lowers the psychological capacity of women for childbearing (Nandi et al. 2017). On the contrary, it is possible that after a very dramatic event, the desire for having another child may instead increase because fertility may take a symbolic meaning and represent a return to normality (Lindstrom and Kiros 2007; Norris et al. 2002; Rodgers et al. 2005).

The effects of conflict on fertility are likely to vary with parity, mother’s age, and time horizon. Yet, theoretical elaborations on these aspects are still lacking. In general, it is expected that women who are younger and childless or at lower parities do, ceteris paribus, desire more children and exhibit stronger behavioral fertility response to child death (Nobles et al. 2015). As for the time horizon, Rodgers et al. (2005) argued that the duration of the effect would depend on the type of event. In particular, terror management theory suggests that the decay of the (positive) effect on fertility depends on how quickly the feelings of threat to life disappear and the situation returns to normality.

The existence of economic, social, physiological, and psychological mechanisms linking child death and fertility suggest that the sign of the effect of child mortality on fertility is theoretically ambiguous and needs to be determined empirically.

Women’s Sibling Mortality

The effect of a sibling’s death on the surviving sibling is also theoretically ambiguous. On the one hand, experiencing the death of a sibling could influence other siblings’ outcomes because of the loss of positive (monetary and nonmonetary) inputs or through bereavement (Stroebe et al. 2006). On the other hand, the death of a sibling reduces competition for parental inputs among surviving siblings (Yi et al. 2015). Finally, a sibling’s death may also reduce parental inputs because of grief. Fletcher et al. (2013) found that experiencing the death of a sibling during childhood influences various adult outcomes and that the cause of the sibling’s death matters. Interestingly, surviving brothers and sisters seem to be differentially affected, with the effect being stronger for surviving females. This result is in line with the fact that women usually report greater intimacy in sibling relationships than men (Kim et al. 2006). Finally, Fletcher et al. (2018) showed that the effects are larger if the surviving sibling is older, suggesting sensitive periods of exposure, whereas the negative effects decline over time.

Changes in Local Demographic Conditions

Conflict may affect fertility by changing the local demographic conditions. In particular, it may influence the marriage market by changing the sex ratio (defined as the relative number of men to women).5 In fact, a conflict-induced imbalance in the sex ratio is expected to negatively affect the marriage market and reduce fertility (Brainerd 2016). For instance, Bethmann and Kvasnicka (2013) showed that in Bavaria, Germany, the decline in the sex ratio induced by World War II increased the proportion of out-of-wedlock childbearing but reduced overall fertility. As for Rwanda, robust evidence shows that the genocide reduced the sex ratio, that the effect was stronger in communes with a higher genocide intensity, and that this affected marital outcomes, domestic violence, and time use (La Mattina 2017; Schindler 2010; Schindler and Verpoorten 2013; Verpoorten 2005).

Data

Our analysis builds on three cross-sectional waves of the Rwandan DHS, collected by ORC Macro and the National Institute of Statistics of Rwanda in 2000, 2005, and 2010. The data in each survey is representative of households in Rwanda, based on a stratified survey design selected in two stages. In the following, all analyses account for the survey design, and population weights are used as recommended by the data providers. In each selected household, all women aged 15–49 who were either usual household members or present in the household on the night before the interview were eligible for interviewing. The questionnaire design remained broadly similar across the survey waves. The sample size increased over time, with 10,421, 11,183, and 13,671 women included in the 2000, 2005, and 2010 survey waves, respectively. Our sample of analysis is restricted to women who were aged 10–45 at the time of the genocide.6 Table 1 reports summary statistics for the main variables we use in the analysis.

The DHS provides detailed information on women’s birth histories; maternal health; child health; marital status; and socioeconomic characteristics, including educational attainment, main occupation, and housing characteristics. The DHS also collects some information on respondents’ partners, including age, education, and occupation. Income and consumption expenditures are not recorded. Therefore, we construct a wealth index based on household assets.7

We employ three alternative proxies for exposure to the genocide: child mortality, women’s sibling mortality, and genocide-induced change in the commune-level sex ratios.

Child Mortality

The DHS questionnaire records child mortality in detail. For each sample woman who has ever lost a child, the month of death, gender, and age of the deceased child is recorded (cause of death is not asked for). This allows us to create a dummy variable, CHILDicw, that takes the value 1 if a woman i living in commune c and interviewed in wave w lost one or more children between April and July 1994 (the period of the genocide), and 0 otherwise. Figure 1 shows the proportion of child deaths relative to the total number of living children reported by sample women for each year during the 1985–2010 period, separately for each DHS wave. The proportion of child deaths peaked during the genocide (increasing by more than twofold relative to the pre-genocide period), returned to pre-genocide levels in 1995, and then started decreasing further. Even though the proportion of child deaths is not low in the pre-genocide period (as is expected for a low-income country like Rwanda), the figure reassuringly shows no evidence of a positive trend in child mortality pre-genocide.

Women’s Sibling Mortality

The DHS questionnaire also records detailed information on each woman’s siblings born to the same mother. For every sibling, information is available on the gender, date of birth, whether the sibling is still alive, year of death, and whether the death is related to pregnancy or childbirth.8 This allows us to create the dummy variable SIBLINGicw, which takes the value 1 if a woman experienced the death of one or more siblings during the genocide, and 0 otherwise. To ensure that this variable captures only those deaths related to the genocide, we exclude all deaths related to pregnancy and childbirth for female siblings. Figure 2 shows the proportion of sibling deaths relative to the total number of living siblings reported by sample women for each year during the 1985–2010 period, calculated separately for all three DHS waves. The graph exhibits a single peak, which coincides with the 1994 genocide.

Genocide-Induced Change in the Commune-Level Sex Ratios

The third conflict proxy is a continuous variable capturing the change in the commune-level demographic conditions caused by the genocide. We construct the variable ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002 as the difference between the pre-genocide and post-genocide sex ratios at the commune level. Data on sex ratios come from two secondary sources: the 1991 census (the most recent population data available from before the genocide) and the 2002 census (the first population census collected after the genocide).9 For each commune, the sex ratio (the ratio of males to females) is calculated for the population aged 15–60. We exclude individuals living in institutions, such as prisons, convents, and military camps. As shown in Fig. 3, the change in the commune-level sex ratio exhibits plenty of spatial variation across the 145 communes included in the analysis. On average, the sex ratio decreased by 15 percentage points, from 0.98 males per female in 1991 to 0.83 males per female in 2002. The sex ratio decreased in all 145 communes, with the value of this reduction ranging from a minimum of 0.002 to a maximum of 0.32. Given that ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002 takes only positive values, larger values reflect larger reductions in the sex ratio.

Estimation Strategy

We employ various estimation strategies to explore the impact of the genocide on fertility. The first is a Cox proportional hazard model to investigate the effects of conflict on the duration time to the first birth after genocide: that is, on the hazard of having a birth after the genocide. The Cox proportional hazard is defined as
hicwt=h0texpβConflicticw+Xicw'γ+δc+θw,
1
where t is the duration (or survival) time, hicw(t) is the hazard rate at time t for woman i living in commune c and interviewed in wave w, and h0(t) is the baseline hazard function that depends on t but not X. The baseline hazard function is assumed to be unknown and is unparameterized.10 The duration time is defined as the number of months between June 199511 and the first birth, if any, occurring up to and including May 2000. We use data on births occurring during the same time window—June 1995 to May 2000—in each DHS wave. The hazard rate at time t is the probability of occurrence of the event “having a birth” at time t if it has not already occurred. Our focus here is on the effects of the genocide on fertility in the short run, during the five years immediately following the genocide. Women having their first post-genocide birth after May 2000 are treated as right-censored observations.12

Conflicticw is a dummy variable capturing a woman’s exposure to violence during the genocide. As a proxy for the type and intensity of exposure to conflict, we use two measures: a dummy variable taking the value 1 if the woman experienced the death of a child during the genocide, and 0 otherwise (CHILDicw); and a dummy variable taking the value 1 if the woman experienced sibling death during the genocide, and 0 otherwise (SIBLINGicw). β is the key parameter of interest to be estimated, which captures the effect of violence exposure on the duration time to the first birth in the five years after genocide—that is, on the hazard of giving birth within the five years following the genocide. Xicw is a matrix of covariates including (1) woman-specific characteristics (age, age squared, marital status at the time of the genocide, education level, and previous fertility),13 (2) household-specific characteristics (wealth index and an indicator for urban residence), and (3) commune-specific characteristics (mortality of children under age 5 during the five years preceding the genocide and the sex ratio before the genocide). Finally, δc is a vector of commune dummy variables, capturing all time-invariant factors at the commune level (commune fixed effects); and θw is a vector of dummy variables for the survey waves (wave fixed effects). Standard errors are clustered at the primary sampling unit (PSU) level.

Next, we perform a survival analysis, using the genocide-induced change in the commune-level sex ratio as conflict measure. In this case, the Cox proportional hazard is defined as
hicwt=h0texpβΔSex ratioc,19912002+πSexratioc,1991+Xicw'γ+δp+θw,
2
where the variable ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002 measures the change in the sex ratio from the pre-genocide period to the post-genocide period in commune c. This variable measures the genocide-induced reduction in the relative number of men in the commune and serves as proxy for a woman’s chance of entering into a relationship. The variable ΔSex ratioc,1991 accounts for the pre-genocide level of the sex ratio in the commune. Xicwis the same matrix of individual-, household-, and commune-specific covariates as in Eq. (1). Finally, δp is a vector of prefecture fixed effects.14 Standard errors are again clustered at the PSU level.
The second estimation strategy is a count data model to determine the effect of the conflict on the total number of post-genocide births. To account for both censoring at 0 and the nonnegative integer nature of the outcome values, we estimate the following Poisson regression model:
Yicw=α+βConflicticw+Xicwγ+δc+θw+uicw,
3
where Yicw denotes the post-genocide fertility of woman i living in commune c and interviewed in wave w. More specifically, the outcome variable contains the number of births a woman had between June 1995 and the date of each survey interview. Pooling all three DHS waves (2000, 2005, and 2010) here means that we consider a different period for when births can occur in each wave. This period is 5 years, 10 years, and 15 years long for the 2000, 2005, and 2010 waves, respectively. Conflicticw is a dummy variable indicating a woman’s exposure to conflict during the genocide, as measured by the two conflict proxies outlined earlier, CHILDicw and SIBLINGicw. Xicwis a matrix of covariates including the same individual-, household-, and commune-specific characteristics as in Eq. (1). Finally, δc represents commune fixed effects, capturing all time-invariant factors at the commune level. θw is a vector of dummy variables for survey waves (wave fixed effects). uicw is the error term. Standard errors are clustered at the PSU level.
Finally, we explore the effects of conflict on the total number of children born after the genocide, using the genocide-induced change in the commune-level sex ratio as proxy. To this end, we estimate the following Poisson regression model:
Yicw=α+βΔSexratioc,1991-2002+πSexratioc,1991+Xicw'γ+δp+θw+uicw,
4
where Yicw denotes the number of children born between June 1995 and the date of interview to woman i living in commune c and interviewed in wave w. As in Eq. (2), ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002 measures the change in the commune-level sex ratio from the pre-genocide period to the post-genocide period. The variable Sex ratioc,1991 measures the pre-genocide level of the sex ratio in each commune. Xicwis the same matrix of individual-, household-, and commune-specific covariates as in Eq. (1). Finally, δp is a vector of prefecture fixed effects.

Results

The Effects of the Genocide on the Duration Time to the First Birth After Genocide

As a first step, we investigate the effects of the genocide on the duration time to the first birth after the genocide—that is, on the hazard of having a birth in the five years following the genocide. Our sample includes all women aged 10–45 at the time of the genocide. As a refinement, we also estimate the effect separately by age cohort, based on a woman’s age at the time of the genocide, and by parity, based on the number of children a woman had before June 1995.

Table 2 reports the results obtained from Cox regressions when we use as measure of conflict exposure CHILDicw, a dummy variable taking the value 1 if a woman experienced the death of a child during the genocide, and 0 otherwise. Column 1 reports the Cox regression coefficients from the baseline specification in which we include only the conflict proxy, the woman’s age (and age squared), commune fixed effects, and wave fixed effects. Results show that for women exposed to child death during the genocide, the estimated coefficient of the conflict variable is 0.28. This corresponds to a 32 % higher hazard of having a birth within five years after the genocide for women who lost a child during the genocide than for women who did not experience child death.15 Thus, the duration time to the event is shorter for women exposed to child death (compared with women not exposed), which is consistent with a replacement effect. As shown in column 2, this result is robust to the inclusion of the full set of control variables. Interestingly, when we look at the results by women’s age cohort (columns 3–5), we find the effect to be highly statistically significant and large in magnitude for older women (aged 20–45 at the time of the genocide), but the effect is not significant for the young cohort (i.e., aged 10–19 at the time of the genocide). Finally, we look at the effects of child death on fertility by parity. The estimates reported in columns 6–8 confirm a significant effect for all parity groups, although the effect is stronger for lower parities. Additional regressions (Table A1, columns 1–2 in the online appendix) show that the effect of child death is significant and positive for both the death of a son and the death of a daughter. This indicates that the replacement effect is at work independent of the lost child’s gender.

Next, we consider the effect of a woman’s exposure to sibling death during the genocide on the duration time to the first birth after genocide. Results are displayed in Table 3. As before, we report the Cox coefficients for the baseline and main specifications (the one with all controls), and by age cohort and by parity. The variable of interest, SIBLINGicw, has a negative coefficient in all specifications, indicating that the duration time to the first birth is longer; that is, the hazard of having a birth is smaller for women exposed to a sibling death than to women not exposed. Yet, the estimated coefficient is statistically significant only in the specification including all controls (column 2) and for the youngest cohort (column 3). We interpret these results as suggesting that the possible negative psychological effect of sibling loss due to the genocide is more likely to affect fertility if the violent event occurs when the woman is young. Interestingly, when disaggregating the effects of sibling death by a sibling’s age relative to each sample woman (Table A1, columns 3–8 in the online appendix), we find that the effect is stronger (i.e., the duration time is longer) for the death of a younger sibling, although it is significant only in the case of the death of a younger brother. This finding is in line with the theoretical predictions suggesting a stronger effect for the death of a younger sibling. Because the death of a younger brother is likely to have occurred while the woman was still living with her parents, this variable may capture the trauma effect of having witnessed violence committed against close family members or stigmatization due to belonging to a victimized household.

As a final step in our survival analysis, we obtain Cox regression coefficients when using the genocide-induced change in the commune-level sex ratio to measure conflict exposure. As shown in Table 4, the coefficient of interest is negative in all specifications. Thus, women exposed to a more severe local shortage of men because of the genocide have a lower hazard of having a birth in the five years after the genocide; that is, the duration time to their first birth after genocide is longer. Moreover, reading across columns 3–5, our results show that (not surprisingly) the negative effect of the decline in the sex ratio on the hazard of having a birth is strongest for the oldest cohort.

Last, we comment on the covariates that turn out to be statistically significant across specifications (Tables 2, 3, and 4). The first is the household wealth index, which, in line with results from previous studies, is significant and negative. This finding suggests that under the assumption that current wealth is predicted by past wealth, the hazard of having a birth is smaller and the duration time to the birth is longer for women from relatively wealthier households. The second variable is the number of children born before June 1995. Its negative sign indicates that the hazard of having a birth after the genocide declines and the duration time to the first post-genocide birth increases with the number of children conceived before the genocide. Finally, we find that the percentage of children ever lost before the genocide is significant and negatively correlated with the hazard of having a birth after the genocide. Yet, the correlation is positive for the youngest cohort. This finding is not surprising because the replacement effect is more likely for those women.

The Effects of the Genocide on the Total Number of Post-Genocide Births

To complement the previous analyses, we now examine the effects of the conflict on the total number of post-genocide births, looking again at the effects of each of the three measures of conflict exposure. As in the survival analysis, our sample includes all women aged 10–45 at the time of the genocide. Again, we also estimate the model for the total number of births after the genocide separately by age cohort, based on a woman’s age at the time of the genocide, and by parity. In addition, we analyze the effects of the genocide on fertility in the short (1995–2000), medium (2000–2005), and long term (2005–2010).

Table 5 reports results obtained from estimating Eq. (3) with a Poisson regression model and when using CHILDicw as a proxy for a woman’s exposure to the genocide. Column 1 displays results for the baseline specification. Results show a strong and positive effect of CHILDicw on the number of births after the genocide, again indicating that a replacement effect is at work. Women who lost at least one child during the genocide have significantly more births in the post-genocide period. This result is robust to the inclusion of the full set of control variables (column 2, the main specification). With respect to the magnitude of the estimated coefficient, we find that having experienced the death of a child during the genocide increases the predicted number of children born after the genocide by 10 %. Column 9 shows that the replacement effect is strong and significant in the short run (i.e., in the five years after the genocide), but the effect is reversed in the long run. Interestingly, we find that although the effect of child death is positive for both son and daughter deaths, it is significant only for a deceased male child (Table A2, columns 1–2, in the online appendix).

Next, we look at the effect of an exposure to sibling death during the genocide on the total number of births in the post-genocide period. To this end, we estimate Eq. (3) using the variable SIBLINGicw as a measure of genocide exposure. Table 6 shows the results. Columns 1 and 2 report the estimates for the baseline and main specifications. The negative coefficients for SIBLINGicw indicate that women who experienced the death of a sibling during the genocide have significantly lower fertility in the post-genocide period than women who did not lose a sibling. The magnitude of the estimated coefficient in column 2 indicates that being exposed to sibling death during the genocide decreases the predicted number of children by 5 %. The analysis by parity (columns 6–9) indicates that although the effect is significant and decreases with an increase in parity, it is not significant for women who did not have children or had three or more children at the end of the genocide. Finally, the analysis of the effect by time horizon (columns 10–12) indicates that although the effect of the death of a sibling is always negative, it is larger in the long run. Interestingly, we find a significant and negative effect for the death of a sibling, irrespective of the sibling’s gender (Table A2, columns 3 and 6 in the online appendix). When we disaggregate sibling death by the age of the sibling relative to the sample women, we find the strongest effect for the death of a younger brother. This finding confirms results from the survival analysis and is in line with theoretical predictions suggesting a stronger effect for the death of a younger (rather than an older) sibling.

Finally, we examine the effect of the conflict-induced change in the commune-level sex ratio on the number of children born after the genocide. Results for Eq. (4) are reported in Table 7. In the baseline (column 1) and main specifications (column 2), the estimated coefficients for ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002 are negative, with the latter being significant at 5 %. This indicates that a genocide-induced decrease in the local sex ratio (a relative reduction in the number of men with respect to the number of women in the commune) lowers the total number of births a woman had after the genocide. In particular, the effect is significant for the oldest cohort and for women with higher parity. Moreover, the effect is significant if we restrict the analysis to the short run (i.e., the five years following the genocide). This confirms the results obtained with survival analysis. Taken together, these results suggest that the genocide affected fertility in the short run by decreasing the possibility of marital matching due to the conflict-induced reduction in the local sex ratio.

Regarding other covariates, we find some to be significant across results in Tables 5, 6, and 7. Both secondary (or higher) education and the number of children born before June 1995 tend to decrease post-genocide fertility. The coefficient for household wealth is always negative and significant. Again, the percentage of children lost before the genocide is negatively correlated with the number of total post-genocide births for the full sample but not for the youngest cohort.

Robustness Tests

We conduct several tests on the robustness of results. First, we reestimate the survival and count data models, this time including all three proxies for conflict exposure simultaneously. Results are reported in Tables A3 and A4, respectively, in the online appendix. Interestingly, results are virtually unchanged for both models compared with those obtained with separate regressions, both in terms of significance levels and effect size. This suggests that the mechanisms captured by the three measures of genocide intensity are not substitutes; rather, it appears that these measures affect fertility decisions independently from one another.

Second, we explore whether our results capture the specific effect of the genocide on fertility or whether our analysis simply picks up an effect that would materialize any time a woman loses a child or a sibling. Recall that in our main analysis, we already control for the percentage of children ever lost before the genocide. We now reestimate the Cox regression using CHILDicw as the genocide measure and adding a dummy variable taking the value 1 if a woman experienced the death of a child during the 1990–1993 period (i.e., before the genocide), and 0 otherwise, while accounting for the full set of control variables. Results in column 1 of Table A5 in the online appendix show that the replacement effect for child death during the genocide rather than in another period is significantly larger,16 suggesting that exposure to the genocide does have a differential effect on fertility outcomes. Next, we conduct a similar test on the effect of sibling death. To this end, we construct a dummy variable taking the value 1 if the sibling death occurred in the 1990–1993 period (i.e., before the genocide), and 0 otherwise. Again, we reestimate our Cox regression using SIBLINGicw as the genocide measure and adding the dummy variable to the full set of controls. Results in column 2 show that the variable for sibling death before the genocide is not statistically different from 0. This evidence confirms that our analysis captures fertility effects that are specific to a woman’s sibling mortality during the 1994 genocide. Comparable results are obtained when carrying out this test with our Poisson regressions (Table A6 in the online appendix). We interpret this evidence as convincingly showing that the effects of child and sibling deaths during the genocide are different from those during normal times. This could suggest that although child death is not a rare event in Rwanda, if child death does not occur for typical reasons (such as health problems and poverty-related issues), then the desire for replacement may be stronger. Moreover, the effect of the genocide on fertility may also be reinforced by the objective of replacing individuals of the same ethnic group who have been killed during the genocide (Heuveline and Poch 2007).

Third, we explore whether results are driven by the choice of the regression sample. Recall that all regressions discussed so far are carried out on the full sample of women aged 10–45 at the time of the genocide. Instead, we now reestimate all regressions based on different samples that are tailored to each conflict proxy. For the analysis of the effects of child death, we restrict the sample to those women who had at least one child before the genocide began. For the analysis of the effects of sibling death, we restrict the sample to those women who had at least one sibling before the genocide. For the analysis of the genocide-induced change in the sex ratio, we restrict the sample to women who had their first marriage after the genocide. Results for the survival model (Table A7 in the online appendix) and for the count data model (Table A8) show that using these restricted samples yields qualitatively similar results to those using the full sample, providing confidence in the robustness of our findings.

Fourth, because our analysis builds on retrospective data, we check whether recall bias is a serious concern. To test for this, we reestimate the main specification in Table 2 (column 2) separately for each DHS wave. Results reported in Table A9 in the online appendix show that the effect of child death on fertility is similar across each DHS wave. We interpret this as evidence supporting that the recall bias is not a major concern for our analysis.

Fifth, we reestimate all main regression specifications with the addition of location-specific linear time trends to capture all time-varying characteristics at the commune and prefecture levels. Results in Table A10 (for the survival analysis) and Table A11 (for the count data model) in the online appendix show that all main results for the effects of conflict—as proxied by CHILDicw, SIBLINGicw, and ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002—are unchanged.

Finally, we explore the possibility that fertility differs across Hutu and Tutsi, examining how this difference may bias our results. Ideally, one would control for ethnicity in all models. Unfortunately, this is not possible because since the genocide, Rwandan law prohibits collecting information on the ethnicity of respondents. Thus, to shed light on the possibility that fertility outcomes differ between the two main ethnic groups, we turn to information included in the pre-genocide survey: namely, the 1992 DHS. Of the nationally representative sample of women surveyed in the 1992 DHS, 8.6 % reported being Tutsi. Descriptive statistics suggest the existence of some differences across ethnic groups. On average, Tutsi women had 0.6 fewer living children, married 1.7 years later, and gave birth to their first child 1.7 years later than Hutu women in 1992. (All three figures are significantly different in means across Hutu and Tutsi.) The other socioeconomic characteristics differing between Hutu and Tutsi are education, the likelihood of residing in urban areas, and wealth. As a first step, we test the effect of being Tutsi on fertility in the pre-genocide period. We do this separately for the 5 years preceding the survey (1987–1992), the 10 years preceding the survey (1982–1992), and the 15 years preceding the survey (1977–1992), using the same set of covariates from our main specification as controls. We find that, ceteris paribus, being Tutsi has no effect on fertility in the 1987–1992 period, although it has a negative and significant effect on fertility in both the 1982–1992 and 1977–1992 periods (Table A12 in the online appendix). This finding implies that because we cannot control for ethnicity, our results may be biased. Yet, the direction of the bias potentially introduced by the omitted Tutsi variable depends on the conflict proxy that we use. When we use SIBLINGicw or ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002, we expect both measures to be negatively associated with fertility and positively associated with the Tutsi indicator because, as discussed earlier, most people killed during the genocide were Tutsi. It follows that, if anything, the estimates obtained when using SIBLINGicw or ΔSex ratioc,1991–2002 are likely to be biased downward. In other words, the estimated effect of conflict on fertility obtained in these two cases is likely to reflect the lower bound of the true effect of conflict, making our results conservative. Instead, in the case of CHILDicw, the direction of the bias is ambiguous. Although child death is positively associated with fertility, being Tutsi is negatively associated with fertility, and the two measures are positively correlated: children from Tutsi households were more likely to have been killed during the genocide. Thus, the sign of the bias depends on which effect dominates—the positive effect of CHILDicw or the negative effect of being Tutsi. Yet, by reading our main results for this mechanism (Tables 2 and 5) together with those of the effect of being Tutsi on fertility, we can derive a clear conclusion. Because ethnicity does not affect fertility in the short run, it is very unlikely that one of our main results—that there is a replacement effect in the five years after the genocide (Table 5, column 9)—is driven by the differing propensity for fertility between Tutsi and Hutu.

Conclusions

In this article, we study the effects of the 1994 genocide in Rwanda on fertility, using detailed individual-level data and various measures of individual exposure to violence. Using both survival analysis and count data models, we investigate the effects of exposure to violence on both the duration time to the first birth in the five years following the genocide (i.e., the hazard of having a child within the first five years after the genocide) and the total number of births in the post-genocide period.

We find evidence that both channels—the type of violence a woman was exposed to and the conflict-induced change in local demographic conditions—influence post-genocide fertility outcomes. On the one hand, the death of a mother’s child during the genocide increases both the hazard of having a child within five years and the total number of births within 15 years following the genocide. This is strong evidence for a replacement effect. At the same time, sibling death during the genocide significantly lowers the hazard of having a child in the five years following the genocide as well as total post-genocide fertility, especially if a woman lost a younger sibling. This suggests the existence of a psychological mechanism. On the other hand, the genocide-induced reduction in the local sex ratio has a strong negative impact on both the hazard of having a child in the five years after the genocide and on total fertility, with the effect being highly significant in the short run and for older women.

Taken together, these results suggest that both the type of violence experienced and the genocide-induced changes in the local demographic conditions matter for post-conflict fertility outcomes. Our analysis also highlights differential effects of the genocide in terms of age cohorts, parity, and time horizon (short, medium, and long run). In particular, our results by age group are informative. The genocide has no effect on the total number of children post-genocide for the youngest age group, whereas all measures of genocide violence have an effect for the older age groups. At the same time, looking at the hazard of having a child during the five years following the genocide, the youngest and oldest age groups are those most affected in their fertility decisions. This heterogeneity in the effects of violent events on fertility suggests the importance of better understanding the precise mechanisms behind the aggregate effects of conflict on demographic changes.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for helpful comments from three anonymous reviewers, Damien de Walque, Quy-Toan Do, Paul Francis, Kathleen Jennings, Adam Lederer, Marinella Leone, Malte Lierl, Tony Muhumuza, Amber Peterman, Susan Steiner, Håvard Strand, Marijke Verpoorten, Philip Verwimp, and Marc Vothknecht. Uuriintuya Batsaikhan provided excellent research assistance. We are indebted to the National Institute of Statistics Rwanda and, in particular, Augustin Twagirumukiza. The study was funded by the World Bank, with generous support from the Government of Norway. Michele Di Maio gratefully acknowledges the financial support from University Parthenope (Programma di Sostegno alla Ricerca Individuale). The usual disclaimer applies.

Notes

1

A commune in Rwanda in 1991 denoted a local administrative unit akin to a district.

2

Caldwell (2004) noted that economic shocks generally have negative short-term effects on fertility.

3

For a detailed account of the historical evolution of the tensions between Hutu and Tutsi, see Prunier (1999), Newbury (1988), Mamdani (2001), and Desforges (1999).

4

In the literature, the term replacement effect includes both the physiological effect (associated with the truncation of lactation and the shortening of the length of the postpartum amenorrheic period) and the volitional replacement effect. The former, however, would apply only to cases of infant death and cannot explain the relation between child death and fertility (Palloni and Rafalimanana 1999).

5

Conflict may affect the marriage market in ways that go beyond the decline in the sex ratio (La Mattina 2017). First, conflict may decrease women’s utility of being unmarried because of deteriorating economic conditions and increased risk of becoming a victim of sexual violence, thus increasing fertility. Second, the genocide may delay the age of first marriage, which would decrease fertility.

6

We retain only women in the analysis sample for whom fertility information is available for at least five years during the post-genocide period. Thus, the age of the women included in our sample is slightly different for each wave. For instance, consider the 2010 wave that interviewed women aged 15–49 in 2010. When we restrict the sample to women aged 10–45 years in 1994, the regression sample consists of women who were aged 25–49 in 2010.

7

Components of the wealth index include durables and housing characteristics. This wealth index provides a proxy for long-term economic well-being because many durables and housing characteristics are typically held by households for many years and are infrequently replaced (Sahn and Stifel 2000).

8

Accuracy tests on the sibling mortality module in the DHS are discussed in de Walque and Verwimp (2010).

9

For comparability, our analysis applies the administrative structure in place in 1991 to all DHS waves, when Rwanda’s administrative structure consisted of 11 prefectures and 145 communes.

10

The duration time is parameterized in terms of the set of covariates, including the conflict proxy, but the particular distributional form of the duration time is not parameterized. Also, there is no constant term; the latter is absorbed in h0(t), which is not directly estimated in the model.

11

Using June 1995 as a starting point allows us to exclude children conceived during the genocide, potentially through rape, from our analysis.

12

The Cox regression analysis includes also right-censored observations, thus overcoming problems associated with censoring and preventing bias in our estimates.

13

Previous fertility is defined as the number of children born before June 1995 and the percentage of children ever lost before the genocide.

14

In Eq. (2), we use prefecture fixed effects (instead of commune fixed effects, as in Eq. (1)) because the sex ratio varies at the commune level.

15

To compute the hazard ratio from the Cox coefficients, the following formula is applied: 100×eβ×1eβ×0eβ×0 where β is the estimated regression coefficient.

16

An F test rejects the null hypothesis that the coefficients are equal, with the p value being .06.

References

Agadjanian, V., Dommaraju, P., & Glick, J. E. (
2008
).
Reproduction in upheaval: Ethnic-specific fertility responses to societal turbulence in Kazakhstan
.
Population Studies
,
62
,
211
233
.
Agadjanian, V., & Prata, N. (
2002
).
War, peace, and fertility in Angola
.
Demography
,
39
,
215
231
.
Akresh, R., & de Walque, D. (
2008
).
Armed conflict and schooling: Evidence from the 1994 Rwandan genocide
(Policy Research Working Paper No. 4606).
Washington, DC
:
World Bank
.
Akresh, R., Verwimp, P., & Bundervoet, T. (
2011
).
Civil war, crop failure, and child stunting in Rwanda
.
Economic Development and Cultural Change
,
59
,
777
810
.
André, C., & Platteau, J. P. (
1998
).
Land relations under unbearable stress: Rwanda caught in the Malthusian trap
.
Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization
,
34
,
1
47
.
Ben-Porath, Y. (
1976
).
Fertility response to child mortality: Micro data from Israel
.
Journal of Political Economy
,
84
,
S163
S178
.
Bethmann, D., & Kvasnicka, M. (
2013
).
World War II, missing men and out of wedlock childbearing
.
Economic Journal
,
123
,
162
194
.
Bousmah, M-a-Q (
2017
).
The effect of child mortality on fertility behaviors is non-linear: new evidence from Senegal
.
Review of Economics of the Household
,
15
,
93
113
.
Brainerd, E. (
2016
).
The lasting effect of sex ratio imbalance on marriage and family: Evidence from World War II in Russia
(IZA Discussion Papers No. 10130).
Bonn, Germany
:
Institute for the Study of Labor
.
Brück, T., & Schindler, K. (
2009
).
The impact of violent conflicts on households: What do we know and what should we know about war widows?
.
Oxford Development Studies
,
37
,
289
309
.
Buvinic, M., Das Gupta, M., Casabonne, U., & Verwimp, P. (
2013
).
Violent conflict and gender inequality: An overview
.
World Bank Research Observer
,
28
,
110
138
.
Cain, M. (
1983
).
Fertility as an adjustment to risk
.
Population and Development Review
,
9
,
688
702
.
Caldwell, J. C. (
2004
).
Social upheaval and fertility decline
.
Journal of Family History
,
29
,
382
406
.
Caldwell, J. C., Reddy, P. H., & Caldwell, P. (
1986
).
Periodic high risk as a cause of fertility decline in a changing rural environment: Survival strategies in the 1980–1983 south Indian drought
.
Economic Development and Cultural Change
,
34
,
677
701
.
Clifford, D., Falkingham, J., & Hinde, A. (
2010
).
Through civil war, food crisis and drought: Trends in fertility and nuptiality in post-Soviet Tajikistan
.
European Journal of Population
,
26
,
325
350
.
Curlin, G. T., Chen, L. C., & Hussain, S. B. (
1976
).
Demographic crisis: The impact of the Bangladesh Civil War (1971) on births and deaths in a rural area of Bangladesh
.
Population Studies
,
30
,
87
105
.
Desforges, A. (
1999
).
Leave none to tell the story: Genocide in Rwanda
.
New York, NY
:
Human Right Watch
.
de Walque, D., & Verwimp, P. (
2010
).
The demographic and socio-economic distribution of excess mortality during the 1994 genocide in Rwanda
.
Journal of African Economies
,
19
,
141
162
.
Elveborg Lindskog, E. (
2016
).
Violent conflict and sexual behavior in Rwanda
.
Population, Space and Place
,
22
,
241
254
.
Fletcher, J., Mailick, M., Song, J., & Wolfe, B. (
2013
).
A sibling death in the family: Common and consequential
.
Demography
,
50
,
803
826
.
Fletcher, J., Vidal-Fernandez, M., & Wolfe, B. (
2018
).
Dynamic and heterogeneous effects of sibling death on children’s outcomes
.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
,
115
,
115
120
.
Frankenberg, E., Laurito, M. M., & Thomas, D. (
2015
).
The demographic impact of disasters
. In J. Wright (Ed.),
International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences
(2nd ed., pp.
101
108
).
Amsterdam, the Netherlands
:
Elsevier
.
Heuveline, P., & Poch, B. (
2007
).
The Phoenix population: Demographic crisis and rebound in Cambodia
.
Demography
,
44
,
405
426
.
Hill, K. (
2004
).
War, humanitarian crises, population displacement, and fertility: A review of evidence
.
Washington, DC
:
National Academies Press
.
Hossain, M. B., Phillips, J. F., & Legrand, T. K. (
2007
).
The impact of childhood mortality on fertility in six rural thanas of Bangladesh
.
Demography
,
44
,
771
784
.
Iqbal, Z. (
2010
).
War and the health of nations
.
Standford, CA
:
Stanford University Press
.
Islam, A., Ouch, C., Smyth, R., & Wang, L. C. (
2016
).
The long-term effects of civil conflicts on education, earnings, and fertility: Evidence from Cambodia
.
Journal of Comparative Economics
,
44
,
800
820
.
Justino, P., & Verwimp, P. (
2013
).
Poverty dynamics, violent conflict, and convergence in Rwanda
.
Review of Income and Wealth
,
59
,
66
90
.
Kalemli-Ozcan, S. (
2003
).
A stochastic model of mortality, fertility, and human capital investment
.
Journal of Development Economics
,
70
,
103
118
.
Khawaja, M. (
2000
).
The recent rise in Palestinian fertility: Permanent or transient?
.
Population Studies
,
54
,
331
346
.
Khawaja, M., Assaf, S., & Jarallah, Y. (
2009
).
The transition to lower fertility in the West Bank and Gaza Strip: Evidence from recent surveys
.
Journal of Population Research
,
26
,
153
174
.
Kidane, A. (
1989
).
Demographic consequences of the 1984–1985 Ethiopian famine
.
Demography
,
26
,
515
522
.
Kim, J-Y, McHale, S. M., Osgood, D., & Crouter, A. C. (
2006
).
Longitudinal course and family correlates of sibling relationships from childhood through adolescence
.
Child Development
,
77
,
1746
1761
.
La Mattina, G. (
2017
).
Civil conflict, domestic violence and intra-household bargaining in post-genocide Rwanda
.
Journal of Development Economics
,
124
,
168
198
.
Lee, R. (
1990
).
The demographic response to economic crisis in historical and contemporary populations
.
Population Bulletin of the United Nations
,
29
,
1
15
.
Lindstrom, D. P., & Berhanu, B. (
1999
).
The impact of war, famine, and economic decline on marital fertility in Ethiopia
.
Demography
,
36
,
247
261
.
Lindstrom, D. P., & Kiros, G-E (
2007
).
The impact of infant and child death on subsequent fertility in Ethiopia
.
Population Research and Policy Review
,
26
,
31
49
.
Lopez, H., & Wodon, Q. (
2005
).
The economic impact of armed conflict in Rwanda
.
Journal of African Economies
,
14
,
586
602
.
Mamdani, M. (
2001
).
When victims become killers: Colonialism, nativism, and the genocide in Rwanda
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton University Press
.
Montgomery, M. R., & Cohen, B. (
1998
).
From death to birth: Mortality decline and reproductive change
.
Washington, DC
:
National Academies Press
.
Nandi, A., Mazumdar, S., & Behrman, J. R. (
2017
).
The effect of natural disaster on fertility, birth spacing, and child sex ratio: Evidence from a major earthquake in India
.
Journal of Population Economics
,
31
,
267
293
.
Newbury, C. (
1988
).
The cohesion of oppression: Clientship and ethnicity in Rwanda 1860–1960
.
New York, NY
:
Columbia University Press
.
Nobles, J., Frankenberg, E., & Thomas, D. (
2015
).
The effects of mortality on fertility: Population dynamics after a natural disaster
.
Demography
,
52
,
15
38
.
Norris, F. H., Friedman, M., Watson, P., Byrne, C., Diaz, E., & Kaniasty, K. (
2002
).
60,000 disaster victims speak: Part I. An empirical review of the empirical literature, 1981–2001
.
Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes,
,
65
,
207
239
.
Nugent, J. B. (
1985
).
The old-age security motive for fertility
.
Population and Development Review
,
11
,
75
97
.
Palloni, A., & Rafalimanana, H. (
1999
).
The effects of infant mortality on fertility revisited: New evidence from Latin America
.
Demography
,
36
,
41
58
.
Pörtner, C. C. (
2001
).
Children as insurance
.
Journal of Population Economics
,
14
,
119
136
.
Preston, S. H. (
1978
).
The effects of infant and child mortality on fertility
.
New York, NY
:
Academic Press
.
Prunier, G. (
1999
).
Rwanda: Le génocide
.
Paris, France
:
Éditions Dagorno
.
Rindfuss, R. R., Reed, J. S., & St. John, C. (
1978
).
A fertility reaction to a historical event: Southern white birthrates and the 1954 desegregation ruling
.
Science
,
201
,
178
180
.
Rodgers, J. L., Craig, A. S. J., & Ronnie, C. (
2005
).
Did fertility go up after the Oklahoma City bombing? An analysis of births in metropolitan counties in Oklahoma, 1990–1999
.
Demography
,
42
,
675
692
.
Rogall, T., & Yanagizawa-Drott, D. (
2014
).
The legacy of political mass killings: Evidence from the Rwandan genocide
.
Cambridge, MA
:
Unpublished manuscript, John F. Kennedy School of Government, Harvard University
.
Sah, R. K. (
1991
).
The effects of child mortality changes on fertility choice and parental welfare
.
Journal of Political Economy
,
99
,
582
606
.
Sahn, D. E., & Stifel, D. C. (
2000
).
Poverty comparisons over time and across countries in Africa
.
World Development
,
28
,
2123
2155
.
Sandberg, J. (
2006
).
Infant mortality, social networks, and subsequent fertility
.
American Sociological Review
,
71
,
288
309
.
Schindler, K. (
2010
).
Who does what in a household after genocide? Evidence from Rwanda
(DIW Discussion Paper No. 1072).
Berlin
:
German Institute for Economic Research
.
Schindler, K., & Verpoorten, M. (
2013
).
Armed conflict, sex ratio and marital outcomes: Evidence from Rwanda
. Unpublished manuscript,
Department of Development and Security, German Institute for Economic Research
,
Berlin, Germany
.
Schultz, T. P. (
1969
).
An economic model of family planning and fertility
.
Journal of Political Economy
,
77
,
153
180
.
Schultz, T. P. (
1997
).
Chapter 8: Demand for children in low income countries
. In M. R. Rosenzweig & O. Stark (Eds.),
Handbook of population and family economics
(Vol.
1
, part A, pp.
349
430
).
Amsterdam, the Netherlands
:
Elsevier
.
Shemyakina, O. (
2011
).
The effect of armed conflict on accumulation of education: Results from Tajikistan
.
Journal of Development Economics
,
95
,
186
200
.
Stroebe, M. S., Folkman, S., Hansson, R. O., & Schut, H. (
2006
).
The prediction of bereavement outcome: Development of an integrative risk factor framework
.
Social Science & Medicine
,
63
,
2440
2451
.
Urdal, H., & Che, C. P. (
2013
).
War and gender inequalities in health: The impact of armed conflict on fertility and maternal mortality
.
International Interactions
,
39
,
489
510
.
Vail, K. E., Juhl, J., Arndt, J., Vess, M., Routledge, C., & Rutjens, B. T. (
2012
).
When death is good for life: Considering the positive trajectories of terror management
.
Personality and Social Psychology Review
,
16
,
303
329
.
Verpoorten, M. (
2005
).
The death toll of the Rwandan genocide: A detailed analysis for Gikongoro Province
.
Population
,
60
,
331
367
.
Verpoorten, M. (
2009
).
Household coping in war- and peacetime: Cattle sales in Rwanda, 1991–2001
.
Journal of Development Economics
,
88
,
67
86
.
Verpoorten, M. (
2012
).
Leave none to claim the land: A Malthusian catastrophe in Rwanda?
.
Journal of Peace Research
,
49
,
547
563
.
Verwimp, P., Justino, P., & Brück, T. (
2009
).
The analysis of conflict: A micro-level perspective
.
Journal of Peace Research
,
46
,
307
314
.
Verwimp, P., Osti, D., & Østby, G. (
2017
).
Migration, forced displacement and fertility during civil war: A survival analysis
(CEB Working Paper 17/016).
Brussels, Belgium
:
Centre Emile Berhnheim, Free University of Brussels
.
Verwimp, P., & Van Bavel, J. (
2005
).
Child survival and fertility of refugees in Rwanda
.
European Journal of Population
,
21
,
271
290
.
Williams, N. E., Ghimire, D. J., Axinn, W. G., Jennings, E. A., & Pradhan, M. S. (
2012
).
A micro-level event-centered approach to investigating armed conflict and population responses
.
Demography
,
49
,
1521
1546
.
Woldemicael, G. (
2008
).
Recent fertility decline in Eritrea: Is it a conflict-led transition?
.
Demographic Research
,
18
,
27
58
. https://doi.org/10.4054/DemRes.2008.18.2.
Wolpin, K. I. (
1997
).
Chapter 10: Determinants and consequences of the mortality and health of infants and children
. In M. R. Rosenzweig & O. Stark (Eds.),
Handbook of population and family economics
(Vol.
1
, part A, pp.
483
557
).
Amsterdam, the Netherlands
:
Elsevier
.
Yanagizawa-Drott, D. (
2014
).
Propaganda and conflict: Evidence from the Rwandan genocide
.
Quarterly Journal of Economics
,
129
,
1947
1994
.
Yi, J., Heckman, J. J., Zhang, J., & Conti, G. (
2015
).
Early health shocks, intra-household resource allocation and child outcomes
.
Economic Journal
,
125
,
F347
F371
.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary data