Abstract

High rates of incarceration among American men, coupled with high rates of fatherhood among men in prison, have motivated recent research on the effects of parental imprisonment on children’s development. We use data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study to examine the relationship between paternal incarceration and developmental outcomes for approximately 3,000 urban children. We estimate cross-sectional and longitudinal regression models that control not only for fathers’ basic demographic characteristics and a rich set of potential confounders, but also for several measures of pre-incarceration child development and family fixed effects. We find significant increases in aggressive behaviors and some evidence of increased attention problems among children whose fathers are incarcerated. The estimated effects of paternal incarceration are stronger than those of other forms of father absence, suggesting that children with incarcerated fathers may require specialized support from caretakers, teachers, and social service providers. The estimated effects are stronger for children who lived with their fathers prior to incarceration but are also significant for children of nonresident fathers, suggesting that incarceration places children at risk through family hardships including and beyond parent-child separation.

Introduction

By the end of 2008, more than 1.5 million individuals were incarcerated in federal or state prisons in the United States, with hundreds of thousands more in local jails (Glaze and Maruschak 2009; Harrison and Beck 2005). An overwhelming majority of these individuals were male, and most had children younger than 18 (Glaze and Maruschak 2009). The large and growing number of incarcerated parents has made understanding the effects of paternal imprisonment on children’s well-being, especially in relation to other forms of father absence, an important and timely goal for social science researchers. High rates of fatherhood among prisoners have also led to policy initiatives designed to reduce the risks posed to children and families by parental incarceration. In 2006, for example, the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services issued 13 grants for programs focused on family strengthening and responsible fatherhood for men in correctional settings (Lindquist and Bir 2008).

Although a substantial literature exists on the intergenerational transmission of criminality, this research focuses on adolescent and adult children of formerly incarcerated parents (Murray and Farrington 2008a). Less is known about the extent to which parental incarceration impacts young children. Moreover, most studies investigating parent incarceration and early child development are limited by small convenience samples and cross-sectional or short-term design. The present study extends previous research, using a longitudinal survey of urban families and a series of statistical models to assess the relationship between fathers’ incarceration and a broad set of child development indicators at age 5. We use cross-sectional and longitudinal regression models that control not only for a rich set of potential confounders, but also for several measures of pre-incarceration child development and family fixed effects.

We use data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study, a population-based sample of urban children. The Fragile Families data include multiple indicators of both incarceration history and child development, as well as a wide range of demographic, socioeconomic, and parent behavioral measures, which help to address omitted variable biases. As a population-based study of families, rather than a sample of inmates or offenders, Fragile Families also provides a large comparison sample of children whose fathers have not been incarcerated. Further, its focus on unmarried parents allows a unique comparison: that of children whose fathers become incarcerated and children whose fathers become absent for other reasons.

Background

Prior research identifies several mechanisms through which parental incarceration may impact young children. First, research drawing on attachment theory (Bowlby 1973) suggests that forced separation can disrupt parent-child bonds, harming children’s social and emotional well-being (Solomon and Zweig 2006; Sroufe 1988). Separation as a result of parental incarceration may be even more detrimental than divorce and other forms of parent-child separation. Most unmarried, nonresident fathers maintain contact with their children (Argys et al. 2006; Tach et al. 2010); and many are involved with daily activities, such as household chores, playing games, and bedtime routines (Waller and Swisher 2006). In contrast, less than one-third of fathers in prison see their children on a regular basis (Hairston 1998). Transportation to prisons can be difficult for families (Arditti et al. 2003; Comfort 2008), and mothers may limit contact between incarcerated fathers and children (Arditti et al. 2005; Edin et al. 2004; Roy and Dyson 2005). Thus, children’s interactions with incarcerated fathers are limited in both quantity and quality, which likely has negative consequences for development (Swisher and Waller 2008).

Second, fathers’ incarceration may impact children through family economic circumstances. The incarceration of a father, even when parents are no longer romantically involved, often leads to decreases in household resources. Pay for work done in prison is meager, and returning offenders are often unable to find work or are relegated to low-paying jobs or the informal economy (Lewis et al. 2007; Western et al. 2001). Families suffer from the loss of fathers’ financial support (Geller et al. 2011; Swisher and Waller 2008) and are at greater risk for material hardship (Schwartz-Soicher et al. 2011). Resource deprivation and any resulting instability are detrimental to family and child well-being (McLoyd 1998).

Third, incarceration may affect children by compromising their parents’ relationship. The economic strain created by incarceration may undermine the father’s traditional role as a provider, straining parents’ relationships (Hairston 1998). The social stigma often associated with incarceration may also disrupt dating relationships, especially among low-income parents (Braman 2004). The ethnographic research of Edin (2000) and Anderson (1999) suggests that poor women weigh heavily the respectability of prospective husbands, and perceive that formerly incarcerated men may threaten family reputation, put mothers’ and children’s safety at risk, and fail to provide a “respectable” middle-class lifestyle. Mothers may also form new relationships while their child’s father is incarcerated, further complicating their relationship with the biological father upon his release (Braman 2004). These qualitative findings reinforce quantitative research reporting that married incarcerated men are more likely than their never-incarcerated counterparts to separate, and that single incarcerated men, especially African Americans, have few marriage prospects upon reentry (Western 2006). The extent to which incarceration places couples at risk for conflict, separation, or divorce may harm children’s development (Amato 2006).

Alternatively, a father’s incarceration may have little or no impact on children. Approximately one-half of fathers behind bars were not living with their children before their incarceration (Johnson and Waldfogel 2002), and the effects of incarceration might be attenuated for children whose contact with their fathers was limited. Additionally, fathers’ incarceration may be less detrimental to daughters, given that fathers are typically less involved with daughters than sons (Lundberg et al. 2007). There are also reasons to suspect that the incarceration of some fathers may improve child well-being by removing a destabilizing influence. For example, if a father is abusive, or if his illegal activities disrupt family relationships or undermine family safety, children may benefit from his incarceration (Whitaker et al. 2006). Jail or prison time may also serve as a “turning point” for some men, in which they resolve to redirect their lives and become better spouses and fathers upon release (Edin et al. 2004). Fathers’ jail or prison experiences may also have a deterrent effect, reducing their or their children’s likelihood of future imprisonment (Edin et al. 2004).

Although research suggests several mechanisms through which fathers’ incarceration may influence children’s development, empirical evidence on the developmental effects of paternal incarceration is limited. The incarcerated population is overwhelmingly young, minority, and poorly educated (Petersilia 2003; Western 2006), and their children face substantial challenges even in the absence of incarceration. Few data are available to isolate the causal effects of incarceration from the confounding effects of family disadvantage. Moreover, many studies are limited by small convenience samples and cross-sectional or short-term design. They therefore provide descriptive information about a sample of children whose parents have been incarcerated but cannot distinguish the challenges faced by these children from those faced by disadvantaged children more generally (see Parke and Clarke-Stewart 2002; and Wilbur et al. 2007).

The handful of studies that examine parental incarceration and child well-being in the context of representative urban and rural populations find children with incarcerated parents to be at serious risk. Children exposed to parental incarceration are more likely to experience financial strain and economic and residential instability (Geller et al. 2009; Phillips et al. 2006), and they are more likely to display aggressive behaviors than their peers (Geller et al. 2009). Although these studies control for a wide range of observable characteristics, they do not account for unobserved characteristics nor assess the role of selection bias (Murray et al. 2009). The recent work of Wildeman (2010) provides greater evidence of causality by focusing on within-family changes to examine the effects of incarceration on child aggression. This research, and that of Wakefield and Wildeman (2011), offers support for the argument that paternal incarceration increases children’s physical aggression. These population-based findings suggest that children of incarcerated fathers are at significant risk for problems during early childhood.

This study grows out of, and extends, the literature in several ways. First, we go beyond prior Fragile Families research on parental incarceration (Geller et al. 2009; Wildeman 2010) by providing detailed comparisons of children whose fathers were absent because of incarceration and those whose fathers were absent for other reasons. Second, we examine a broader range of child development outcomes than most studies, including not only behavioral problems but also physical health, verbal ability, and attention problems. Third, we base our analysis on a comprehensive measure of incarceration history and examine the sensitivity of our results to varying measurement assumptions. Finally, we employ several statistical methods to assess the role of selection, observed and unobserved, in the relationship between paternal incarceration and child development.

Data

The analysis is based on data from the Fragile Families study, which follows a cohort of nearly 5,000 couples with children born between 1998 and 2000 in 20 large U.S. cities (Reichman et al. 2001). The study systematically oversamples unmarried parents, but when weighted or regression-adjusted, it is nationally representative of urban families with children. Both mothers and fathers are surveyed at the time of their child’s birth, with follow-up surveys conducted when the children are 1, 3, and 5 years old.

Measures

Paternal Incarceration

It is well known that individuals underreport illegal and stigmatizing behavior (Groves 2004). There is also some direct evidence of underreporting of incarceration (Golub et al. 2002). A unique strength of the Fragile Families data is the use of multiple sources to identify incarceration. Our measure of fathers’ incarceration includes both father and mother reports, which are supplemented by disposition data and by indirect reports. Fathers are asked to self-report whether they have been charged with a crime in the years leading up to the interview; if so, they are asked whether they have been convicted, and if so, they are asked whether they have been incarcerated. Because of an error in survey development, parents are asked to self-report whether they have been charged and convicted between years 3 and 5 but are not asked to self-report incarceration. The vast majority (2,930) of fathers report not being charged or convicted, implying a report of no incarceration. Of the 209 men indicating a conviction, 165 are confirmed as having been incarcerated by a partner report, disposition data, or an indirect report, and another 30 are confirmed as not having been incarcerated by a partner report. Only 14 are left with ambiguous incarceration status.

Father self-reports are enhanced by “disposition data” recorded by the survey subcontractors, indicating whether a father was incarcerated at the time that they contacted him for follow-up.1 The disposition data identify 121 additional incarcerated fathers between baseline and year 3, and another 122 incarcerated fathers at year 5.

Mothers report at years 1 and 3 whether the father has ever been incarcerated, and at year 5 whether he has been incarcerated in the past two years. Finally, parents’ direct reports and disposition data are supplemented with “indirect reports” of incarceration, in which they cite incarceration as a reason why the father was separated from their child or unable to find a job, or other ways that incarceration affected their lives. Few fathers with incarceration histories were identified from indirect reports alone (6% of those reporting any incarceration before year 5, and 19% of those reporting incarceration between years 3 and 5).

In total, 2,043 fathers are reported as having been incarcerated at some point before their child’s fifth birthday, including 821 reported as incarcerated between the third- and fifth-year surveys. The source for each report is provided in Table 1.

Of the 2,295 couples in which both partners are asked about the father’s criminal history, their reports coincide more than 80% of the time (with both parents reporting incarceration 25% of the time, and both reporting no incarceration 56% of the time.) As expected, most discrepancies between mother and father reports are cases for which she reports incarceration but he does not. This discrepancy could result from either deliberate underreporting or from the survey skip pattern. Fathers are asked to self-report arrest and conviction, and are asked about incarceration only if they report having been convicted. Mothers, on the other hand, are simply asked whether the father has spent time in jail or prison. As a result, mothers might include fathers’ time spent in jail awaiting trial, which would not be included in fathers’ reports if he were not ultimately convicted. Because even short incarceration spells have the potential to compromise labor market performance or destabilize family relationships, we consider fathers to have been incarcerated if either parent reports his incarceration. We measure whether a father was ever incarcerated and whether he was incarcerated between years 3 and 5.

Child Development

Children’s behavioral problems are measured with the Child Behavioral Checklist (Achenbach and Rescorla 2000). For each checklist item, mothers reported the extent to which statements about the child’s behavior are true (0 = not true, 1 = sometimes or somewhat true, 2 = often or very true). The aggression subscale (α = .82) is the sum of mother responses to statements about children’s aggressive behavior (e.g., attacks others, screams, sulks, is suspicious, teases, argues, bullies, is disobedient at school, is disobedient at home, destroys others’ things, destroys own things, fights, threatens, or is unusually loud). Internalizing behavior problems (α = .68) are the sum of children’s scores on the anxious/depressive and withdrawn behavior subscales. The anxious/depressive subscale measures whether children feel overly guilty, self-conscious, worried that no one loves them, worried that they might think or do something bad, worried that they have to be perfect, or worried in general. The withdrawn subscale measures whether children are uninvolved in social activities, are secretive, are shy, are underactive, prefer to be alone, or refuse to talk. Attention problems include five items that assess whether children do poor schoolwork, stare blankly, are confused, daydream, or act without thinking (α = .56). We retained this composite despite its low reliability to maintain consistency with recent Fragile Families research on child behavior (e.g., Meadows et al. 2007) and because the items were designed to be used together. Children’s verbal ability is measured with age-standardized scores on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R). The PPVT-R, a measure of receptive vocabulary, assesses the size and range of words that children understand. Finally, child health is a dichotomous variable based on mother reports, taking a value of 1 for “excellent” or “very good” health, and 0 for “good,” “fair,” or “poor” health.

Table 2 presents the outcomes at age 5 for children whose fathers have been incarcerated and for their counterparts whose parents have never been incarcerated. Children of incarcerated fathers score significantly higher on measures of aggression and attention problems, and significantly lower on the verbal ability measure than their peers. In contrast, the two groups are statistically indistinguishable on measures of health and internalizing problems.

Confounding Covariates

Although the observed challenges experienced by children whose parents have been incarcerated are pronounced and statistically significant, these children’s families also differ on many other dimensions that may influence parental incarceration and child development. We therefore include a detailed set of family-level control variables in our analyses. First, in all analyses, we control for maternal incarceration in the time period of interest. Men with incarceration histories are significantly more likely to partner with women who have also been to jail or prison (Geller et al. 2009), and the incarceration of a mother may also have significant implications for child well-being (Murray et al. 2009; Parke and Clarke-Stewart 2002). Although we leave a detailed examination of maternal incarceration for future research, controlling for this history helps to isolate the effects of fathers’ experiences.

We also identify a number of other demographic and socioeconomic factors, listed in Table 3, that are related to both incarceration risk and child well-being, and assess differences on these measures between families with and without paternal incarceration. The first set of covariates are those established early in the lives of both parents and include demographic characteristics such as race, immigrant background, and family history, as well as behavioral traits such as cognitive ability and impulsivity, which are linked by control theorists to criminal activity (Farrington 1998; Gottfredson and Hirschi 1990) and also have the potential to compromise family processes tied to child development (Dickman 1990). We define family history as whether each parent was living with his or her two biological parents at age 15, and whether each parent’s own mother had a history of mental health problems. Each parent’s cognitive ability is measured with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (Wechsler 1981), and impulsivity is measured with the Dickman (1990) scale of dysfunctional impulsivity. Although the measures were administered during follow-up data collections, they are considered stable constructs, unlikely to be affected by previous incarceration spells (Deary et al. 2004; Moeller et al. 2001). If, however, impulsivity and cognitive ability are altered by the incarceration experience, including them in the analyses will underestimate the effects of incarceration.

The second set of covariates contains those observed at or around the time of the focal child’s birth. These include parents’ age and education as well as a rich set of employment, behavioral, and family characteristics. We control for parents’ relationship status at the time of the child’s birth (married, cohabiting, or nonresident) because unmarried men are at greater risk for criminal behavior (Sampson and Laub 1990) and because children born to unmarried parents tend to face developmental disadvantages (Wu and Wolfe 2001). We also control for whether the mother was living in poverty at the time of the birth because economic disadvantage is associated with incarceration risk and developmental problems (Crosnoe and Cooper 2010; Geller et al. 2009; McLoyd 1998). In addition, we control for several factors reflecting parents’ labor market potential, health, and substance-use patterns. Each is associated with incarceration risk (Western 2006) and with parenting capacity (Eiden et al. 2007; Kahn et al. 2002), which in turn have implications for child well-being. Finally, because child gender, birth order, and low birth weight have been tied to several child development indicators (Aarnoudse-Moens et al. 2009; McHale et al. 2009), we include indicators for whether the focal child is male, was a firstborn, or had low birth weight.

The covariates in our models are valuable given that few surveys of incarceration include such a wide array of descriptors. Circumstances at the time of the focal child’s birth, however, may be endogenous to incarceration. Men enter our sample upon the birth of a child, but among men who have been to jail or prison, their median reported age of first incarceration is 20, an average of six years before the focal child’s birth. To the extent that earlier incarceration precludes men from fatherhood or education, or affects their relationship or other characteristics at the child’s birth, models including these covariates may underestimate the true effect of having been to jail or prison. To guard against this endogeneity, several of our analyses focus on incarceration spells that follow the focal child’s first birthday.

Table 3 also presents family socioeconomic differences by fathers’ incarceration history. As the table shows, children whose fathers have been incarcerated are significantly more likely to be racial and ethnic minorities (although less likely to have immigrant parents), and their parents are more impulsive, score lower on tests of cognitive ability, are less likely to have grown up with both of their parents, and are more likely to have a family history of mental health problems. Children of ever-incarcerated fathers are also born to younger and less-educated mothers, and their parents are less likely to be married or cohabiting. They are also more likely to be born into economic hardship: their mothers are more likely to be in poverty at the time of the birth, both of their parents are less likely to be employed, and their fathers earn significantly less. Their mothers are less likely to be in good health, more likely to have histories of incarceration, and more likely to report domestic violence. Both their parents engage in significantly more risky behaviors such as drug use or heavy drinking. These circumstances are likely to place their children at risk of developmental challenges, and the differences observed in Table 2 thus cannot, on their face, be attributed to the incarceration experience itself. In the sections that follow, we work to isolate the effect of paternal incarceration from the confounding effects of other factors.

Modeling Strategy

We begin our analysis by examining differences between children whose fathers have been incarcerated and those whose fathers have not, and progressively reducing the likelihood that these differences are caused by other family characteristics, observed or unobserved. We then test whether the estimated effects of incarceration are significantly worse for children than the effects of other father absence. In each wave, behavioral outcomes are standardized to a mean of 0 and variance of 1.

Incarceration and Child Well-being

Each outcome is first examined using four multiple regression models. The first model is cross sectional and assesses the association between fathers’ lifetime incarceration history (i.e., whether they have ever been incarcerated) and each outcome, controlling for the family background characteristics noted in Table 3. The controls isolate the relationship between incarceration and child well-being from the confounding effects of family structure, socioeconomic status, and other observable parental characteristics, including mothers’ incarceration.2 Recalling the “early-life” and “established in adulthood” covariate classifications in Table 3, the first model takes the following form:
formula
(1)

To the extent that the covariates established in adulthood might be affected by earlier incarceration (if, for example, a juvenile incarceration limits educational attainment or delays childbearing), the estimates of the “incarceration effect” in Model 1 are likely to be underestimated. On the other hand, if early incarceration and these covariates are caused by personal characteristics not captured in the data, the Model 1 estimates might be overestimated.

To resolve this ambiguity, we estimate a second model, which controls for the same covariates as Model 1 but focuses on fathers’ incarceration between the third- and fifth-year surveys. This model assures that all covariates were measured before the period of incarceration; any remaining relationship between incarceration and child well-being is unlikely to be confounded by these observed characteristics.
formula
(2)
To further isolate the effects of paternal incarceration, we estimate a third model, examining the relationship between child well-being and parental incarceration between years 3 and 5, net of observed socioeconomic controls, but also controlling for levels of child well-being at year 3, prior to the period of incarceration. Examining child development before and after a parental incarceration, particularly compared with the well-being measures of children whose parents were not incarcerated, increases our confidence that changes in child well-being are caused by the incarceration experience rather than by pre-incarceration circumstances.
formula
(3)
Nonetheless, the possibility remains that some unmeasured change in circumstances between years 3 and 5 may have caused both an incarceration and a decline in child well-being, or that some unmeasured difference between families may account for the change in child well-being. To account for these possibilities, we estimate a fourth model, using individual fixed effects to control for time-invariant family characteristics, and for both parents’ incarceration between years 3 and 5. The possibility remains that an unobserved change in family well-being drives the observed relationship: for example, an improvement in maternal stress may reduce the likelihood that she reports her partner as incarcerated, and improve her perception of her child’s behavior. However, the focus on within-family changes limits the influence of cross-sectional heterogeneity.
formula
(4)

Incarceration and Father Absence

To assess the extent to which a father’s incarceration creates more risks for his children than other forms of father absence, we reestimate Models 3 and 4 to examine the relationships between our child well-being measures and both father incarceration and other forms of father absence. Specifically, we identify families in which the father is not reported as having been incarcerated between years 3 and 5 but the parents are living apart at year 5. A father is also considered to be absent if he reports at year 5 that the child spends no time with him, or the mother reports that they are not living together even “some of the time.” In addition to the 821 fathers we identify as incarcerated between years 3 and 5, we identify 1,339 fathers absent for reasons other than incarceration.

Replicating Model 3, we estimate
formula
(5)
and replicating Model 4, we estimate
formula
(6)

We take the “incarceration” and “other absence” coefficients β1 and β2 as the estimated effects of each experience. The reference group in these models now consists of families experiencing neither an incarceration nor other absence, and is thus less disadvantaged than the reference group in the earlier models; we anticipate that the β1 coefficient will therefore be of larger magnitude. We then test for the equality of the β1 and β2 coefficients; rejection of the null hypothesis in these tests suggests that the effect of fathers’ incarceration differs significantly from that of other forms of absence.

Differential Effects of Paternal Incarceration

As noted by Western and Wildeman (2009), the effects of a father’s incarceration on his child’s well-being likely depend on the relationship that the father and his family had prior to his incarceration. We thus reestimate Models 3 and 4, dividing our sample by two key indicators of the relationship between children and their fathers. First, we estimate the models separately for those fathers living with their partner and child at year 3 and those who were nonresident. We anticipate that any damaging effects of fathers’ incarceration will be stronger for children living with their fathers before his incarceration. Second, we estimate the models separately for those families where the mother reports domestic violence (i.e., that the father has hit, slapped, or injured her) at any time by the three-year survey, and those families with no indication of domestic violence. We anticipate that children of abusive fathers experience less harm from their incarceration.

Further, we examine the extent to which the effects of fathers’ incarceration on children differ by child gender. Fathers are typically less involved with daughters than sons (Lundberg et al. 2007), and examinations of paternal incarceration and young children (Geller et al. 2009; Wildeman 2010) suggest that the effects of incarceration on children’s aggressive behavior are limited to boys. In this analysis, we examine whether observed effects are stronger for boys (or girls), replicating Models 3 and 4 with the sample divided by child gender. For each set of comparisons, we perform Chow tests (Greene 2003) to assess the differences in the predictors of child well-being across subgroups.

Sensitivity Analysis

Finally, we test the robustness of our findings to our choice of incarceration measure and to alternative model specifications. As shown in Table 1, we take our measure of incarceration from a mixture of fathers’ self-reports, mothers’ reports about the fathers, disposition data from the survey subcontractors, and indirect indicators of incarceration. To examine the importance of supplementing father reports with mother reports, we reestimate Models 3 and 4 but limit our measure of incarceration to those directly reported by fathers, or indicated in the fathers’ disposition records. This approach identifies far fewer fathers as having been incarcerated: 956 at any time before the year-5 survey (as opposed to 2,043 in the main measure) and 240 between years 3 and 5 (versus 821 in the main measure). Others identified as incarcerated in the main measure are either considered not to have been incarcerated in this analysis (417 in the “ever-incarcerated” measure, and 362 in the “incarcerated between years 3 and 5” measure), or considered to have unknown incarceration status (670 in the “ever-incarcerated” measure, and 219 in the “incarcerated between years 3 and 5” measure). The vast majority of men with unknown status are not interviewed in at least one wave. To further assess the value of partner reports, we also estimate a model that separately examines the effects of father-indicated incarcerations and incarcerations indicated only by mother or indirect reports.

Finally, for those outcomes for which our models suggest a significant effect of incarceration, we perform a falsification test (a variation on Kaushal 2007) to ensure that the observed relationships are not the result of unobserved selection. We run regression models using incarceration between years 3 and 5 to predict child well-being at year 3. Because of the temporal ordering of the variables, incarceration between years 3 and 5 could not feasibly cause an outcome difference at the third-year survey, before the focal incarceration. A significant relationship in these models would therefore suggest that some unobserved characteristic of families experiencing incarceration is driving the observed relationships. A null relationship, on the other hand, would increase confidence that the observed relationship between incarceration and child development at age 5 is due to a causal effect.

Results

Effects of Incarceration

Table 4 presents our regression results examining the relationship between paternal incarceration and the first of our child outcomes, aggressive behavior. Model 1 indicates that children of fathers with incarceration histories display significantly more aggression than their counterparts whose fathers were never incarcerated, above and beyond those associated with other family circumstances. As shown in Model 2, this relationship remains significant, and in fact increases in magnitude, when focusing on recent incarcerations (i.e., between the three- and five-year surveys). Models 3 and 4 provide more stringent tests by controlling for pre-incarceration levels of child behavior and by focusing exclusively on within-child changes in behavior problems following their father’s incarceration. Model 3 suggests that children whose fathers become incarcerated display significantly more aggressive behaviors following his incarceration, above and beyond that predicted by their prior behavior. Likewise, Model 4 shows significant increases in aggression following a father’s incarceration, net of all time-invariant family characteristics.

Models 1–3 also suggest a number of other family circumstances that are significantly tied to children’s aggression. Many of these circumstances are closely linked, and collinearity between our covariates complicates their substantive interpretation. For example, children born to mothers in poverty display more aggressive behaviors at age 5 (p  <  .05). However, children whose fathers earned higher wages at baseline also display significantly more aggression at age 5. A detailed discussion of the economic predictors of child behavior is beyond the scope of this analysis, but the significant effects of paternal incarceration on aggression are robust to their inclusion. Likewise, children’s aggression problems are significantly predicted by parental impulsivity; however, Table 4 suggests that fathers’ incarceration increases aggression above and beyond the level that the covariates would predict.

Maternal incarceration, on the other hand, is not significantly related to child aggression at age 5 above and beyond the increase in behavior problems associated with fathers’ incarceration. This finding likely reflects the relative rarity of maternal incarceration, and the fact that most children whose mothers become incarcerated also have fathers with incarceration histories. The insignificant effect of mother incarceration, when father incarceration is controlled for, is consistent with that found in Geller et al. (2009).

Table 5 presents the paternal incarceration coefficients detailed in Table 4, as well as coefficients summarizing the effects of paternal incarceration on children’s internalizing behaviors, attention problems, mother-reported health, and verbal ability. The columns again represent the findings of Models 1–4, each representing a progressively stricter test of causality.

As in Table 4, the first row of Table 5 suggests a robust effect of paternal incarceration on child aggression. Internalizing behaviors, on the other hand, show no significant effect of fathers’ incarceration. Children’s attention problems are not significantly related to their fathers’ lifetime incarceration (as shown in Model 1); however, fathers’ recent incarceration is significantly associated with increases in children’s attention problems, as shown in Models 2 and 3. This increase is robust to controls for children’s pre-incarceration attention problems; however, the fixed-effects control in Model 4 suggests no within-individual change in attention problems following a father’s incarceration.

Verbal ability, which Table 2 suggests is lower among children of ever-incarcerated fathers, is not significantly related to paternal incarceration after family covariates are controlled for. Finally, mother’s reports of child health are statistically indistinguishable between children whose fathers were incarcerated and those whose fathers were not incarcerated.

Incarceration and Other Father Absence

Table 6 compares the estimated effects of incarceration with the effects of other father absence, using a lagged dependent variable (Model 5) and fixed-effects framework (Model 6). Like the results of Table 5, Table 6 suggests significant damaging effects of incarceration on child aggression and, in Model 5, on children’s attention problems.

Model 5 also suggests that children who lived apart from their fathers for other reasons were harmed by the experience, scoring significantly higher than the reference group on scales of aggression and attention problems. In addition, Model 5 suggests that father absence reduces children’s scores on the PPVT. Model 6, on the other hand, suggests no significant effect of father absence on any outcomes of interest.

Comparing the two disadvantaged groups, Model 5 suggests that the effects of fathers’ incarceration on child aggression and attention problems differ significantly from the effects of father absence. Model 6 also suggests that the effects of incarceration on children’s aggression are worse than the effects of other father absence—although the difference in coefficients is only marginally significant (p  <  .10)—but also suggests no significant effects of incarceration or absence on children’s attention problems.

Differential Effects of Paternal Incarceration

Although Tables 4, 5 and 6 suggest strong and robust effects of incarceration on children’s aggression and provide some evidence of effects on children’s attention problems, Table 7 tests the extent to which the effects may be moderated by families’ pre-incarceration relationships and shows differential effects by fathers’ pre-incarceration residence. Table 8 shows differential effects by domestic violence history, and Table 9 shows differential effects by child gender.

Table 7 suggests that the effects are strongest for those who lived with their fathers in the period leading up to incarceration. Incarceration’s effects on aggressive behavior are stronger and more significant for children whose fathers were resident at the three-year follow-up survey. Likewise, Model 3 suggests that the attention problems associated with incarceration are greater for children whose fathers were resident prior to incarceration, although effects are not significant among either group in Model 4. In each model, Chow tests suggest significant differences in the predictors of child well-being between fathers who were and were not resident prior to incarceration.

Table 8 suggests that although incarceration, on average, has damaging consequences for child development, these consequences are not consistently observed in families in which the father was violent before his incarceration. Model 3 suggests significant effects of incarceration on children’s aggression and attention problems if their fathers were not violent in the years before his incarceration, but no effects in families for which the mother reported domestic violence. Model 4, on the other hand, suggests significant effects on aggression for both groups, but no significant effects on attention for either. The structure of each model varies significantly by domestic violence history. Finally, Table 9 shows that, as found by Wildeman (2010), the effects of paternal incarceration are stronger for sons than they are for daughters. The effects on aggression are nearly twice as large for boys than for girls, and effects on attention problems are limited to boys. Chow tests again suggest structural differences in the predictors of well-being by child gender. However, even limiting the analysis to girls, Model 3 indicates a significant damaging effect of incarceration on aggression, suggesting that a policy focus limited to sons of incarcerated fathers would be misguided.

Sensitivity Analyses

Our sensitivity analyses examine the robustness of our conclusions to our choice of incarceration measure and to alternative modeling strategies. Table 10 replicates Models 3 and 4, both using our more limited measure of fathers’ incarceration, based predominantly on self-reports, and examining the limited measure in conjunction with the incarceration measures based on maternal and indirect reports. The top panel reproduces our results from Table 5. The center panel presents incarceration coefficients from the same models but using only the father-based report.3 As expected, our findings are quite sensitive to our choice of incarceration measure, and two of the three significant relationships observed in Table 5 are not present using the more limited measure of father incarceration. The bottom panel of Table 10 presents model results based on both the father-based incarceration report and the supplemental reports of incarceration based on mother and indirect reports. The effects of mother-reported incarceration more closely resemble the estimates based on the more comprehensive measure: we observe increased levels of aggression problems in families in which the mother reports father incarceration, controlling for fathers’ self-reported incarceration. The converse is not the case; we see no significant relationship between self-reported incarceration and child aggression. In view of the fact that the mother reports identify nearly three-quarters of the incarcerations in the three- to five-year period, this is not surprising. Both father and mother reports of incarceration predict children’s attention problems in Model 3, but neither does in Model 4.

In sum, we find that self-reported incarceration is underreported, that relying solely on self-reports would seriously underestimate the negative effects of incarceration on children, and that partner reports ameliorate both the underreporting and the underestimation of negative effects. Because partners are also likely to underreport incarcerations (Caspi et al. 2001), it is likely that we are underestimating the prevalence of incarceration in our sample and the ill effects of incarceration on children. On the other hand, it is possible that some mothers who believe their partners are bad parents incorrectly identify them as having been incarcerated. This could lead to an overestimate of the incarceration’s ill effects. Given the well-documented underreporting of illegal and stigmatizing behaviors, we expect that supplementing self-reports with partner reports offers findings closer to the truth; however, official incarceration data would help to adjudicate the issue.

Finally, we run falsification tests for children’s aggression and attention problems, the outcomes for which Table 5 indicates significant effects in Model 3. The first row of numbers in Table 11 replicates the Table 5 results, and the second row provides the results of the falsification test. The temporal ordering of incarceration and behavior in this model suggests that there could not be a causal effect of later incarceration on year-3 behavior; a significant relationship in these models would therefore suggest that the relationship between incarceration and child behavior was driven by unobserved selection.

As Table 11 shows, neither aggression nor attention problems at year 3 is significantly predicted by paternal incarceration in the two years that follow. This is consistent with the idea that the relationship between incarceration and these problems is causal, since the associations with child behavior are evident in the period following the incarceration of interest but not in the period preceding it. If, on the other hand, unobserved family characteristics were behind the relationship, they would also have likely induced a relationship in Table 11.

Discussion and Conclusions

Summary of Findings and Implications for Policy and Practice

The increased incidence of incarceration in the United States since the 1970s has led to an unprecedented number of parents in the nation’s prisons and jails (Glaze and Maruschak 2009; Murray et al. 2009). The prevalence of paternal incarceration, in particular, has raised concerns about how children fare when separated from their imprisoned fathers. Given the importance of early experiences for developmental trajectories (Lindquist and Bir 2008; Pianta et al. 2007), understanding the ways in which paternal incarceration affects young children’s health and well-being can inform prevention and intervention efforts. The present study addresses this issue by assessing the relationship between fathers’ incarceration and a broad set of child development indicators at age 5, and eliminating several selection-driven explanations for observed relationships.

Overall, our results suggest that paternal incarceration has significant and damaging consequences for the socioemotional well-being of young children. In a series of cross-sectional, longitudinal, and fixed-effects regression models, we find a robust relationship between incarceration and child aggression. Although the estimates range in magnitude across models, they are consistently significant, robust to controls for several indicators of observable and unobservable heterogeneity, and of sizes comparable to or larger than those of other socioeconomic factors, including maternal education and parents’ baseline relationship status. Moreover, we find some evidence that incarceration has a stronger effect on children’s aggressive behaviors than other forms of father absence. These findings are in line with prior research (Wakefield and Wildeman 2011; Wildeman 2010) and provide additional support for an effect of incarceration on child aggression.

Our analyses also suggest that fathers’ incarceration is significantly associated with increases in children’s attention problems, although these relationships are more sensitive to model specification. After attention problems at age 3 are controlled for, children exposed to paternal incarceration display more attention problems at age 5 than those whose fathers do not become incarcerated. A placebo test suggests that this relationship is not driven by unobserved selection; however, fixed-effects models show virtually no relationship between incarceration and attention problems, suggesting fragility in our estimates.

We find no relationship between incarceration and children’s internalizing problems, verbal ability, or mother-reported health. The findings for internalizing problems run counter to those reported by Wakefield and Wildeman (2011), but the Wakefield and Wildeman discussion of internalizing problems is based on a sample that includes adolescents and young adults as well as children, while our findings are based on a sample of 5-year-old children. Additionally, our measures of children’s cognitive development and health are limited. Future research should therefore examine the developmental consequences of incarceration across various states of the early life course, using a more comprehensive set of outcome measures.

Supplemental analyses indicate that the effects of incarceration are not evenly distributed across families. Consistent with research on the developmental importance of father contact (Swisher and Waller 2008), our results suggest that incarceration elevates behavior problems substantially more for children who had been living with their fathers prior to imprisonment. Yet, incarceration significantly increases attention and aggression problems among children whose fathers were nonresident, suggesting that effects operate at least partially through channels unrelated to father-child contact (e.g., maternal mental health, family economic well-being, or genetic transmission). We also find that estimates of the effects of incarceration on aggression are almost twice as large for boys as for girls, although the effects are significant for both genders. The results for incarceration in contexts of domestic violence are less clear. Although the effect of incarceration is not significant in the lagged model for children exposed to domestic violence, the coefficient is large and similar in magnitude to that for children in nonviolent homes.

These findings suggest the need for a nuanced treatment of paternal incarceration by policymakers and children’s caregivers. If, for example, a father has a history of domestic violence, his children’s greatest challenges may stem from circumstances that preceded the incarceration, and resources may be best spent helping the family recover from abuse. In contrast, problems faced by children in nonviolent homes may relate more directly to their father’s incarceration. Caregivers and service providers working with these children may need to address issues related to diminished father-child contact during his sentence and family stresses that can continue after his release.

A variety of proposals and programs have been developed with the potential to address each of these challenges. Advocates have proposed family-friendly visitation policies and have suggested reductions in the cost of contact between incarcerated individuals and their families (New York Times 2009). Parenting programs, sometimes combined with services such as job training or drug treatment, have also been designed to strengthen family bonds after incarceration (Lindquist and Bir 2008). Additionally, transitional jobs programs may increase earnings and reduce recidivism among formerly incarcerated men (Bloom 2006; Jacobs and Western 2007).

Limitations and Directions for Future Research

The results of this study suggest significant negative consequences of paternal incarceration for children’s development, but we interpret our findings with caution. First, while we control for a wide range of potential confounders of the relationship between incarceration and child development, challenges remain in inferring causal effects from observational data. Unobserved changes in mothers’ or families’ circumstances may have driven both reports of fathers’ incarceration and changes in child aggression. Second, despite the population-based nature of the Fragile Families data, generalizability may be limited by sample attrition. Families observed at the year-5 survey likely differ in unobserved ways from those families who could not be contacted for follow-up. Nonetheless, response rates are high, and given the prevalence of paternal incarceration, we anticipate that our findings have serious implications for children of incarcerated fathers.

In addition, robustness checks suggest that our findings are sensitive to measurement choice and that observed effects are driven by men whose incarceration histories are reported by their partners. However, because we rely on mother reports of the primary independent (paternal incarceration) and dependent variables (child behavior problems), our results may be affected by shared method variance (Bank et al. 1990). If so, the observed effects of fathers’ incarceration on behavior problems are potentially inflated. We therefore examined independent ratings of child temperament and behavior by interviewers during in-home interviews and found that interviewer ratings corroborated maternal reports of child behavior. Consistent with prior Fragile Families research (Meadows et al. 2007), children rated by interviewers as being least cooperative had higher problem behavior scores. These findings do not rule out shared method variance but increase our confidence in mothers’ appraisals of child behavior.

Our analysis, and the study of parental incarceration more generally, would benefit greatly from supplementing survey data on criminal history with administrative reports of respondents’ criminal records, as done in several studies reviewed by Murray and colleagues (2009), such as the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development (Murray and Farrington 2005, 2008a, b). These studies, however, are largely conducted outside the United States. An administrative supplement to an American data set examining parental incarceration would greatly advance our understanding not only of the effects of parental involvement in the criminal justice system but of the reliability of criminal history survey data more broadly.

Future research is also needed to understand the mechanisms governing incarceration effects. Our results suggest that at least a portion of incarceration’s damage is tied to the separation of fathers from their families. It is also well established that incarceration compromises families’ economic stability and parents’ romantic relationships (Western 2006), but the extent to which these factors explain observed effects is not clear. Additionally, research should examine whether fathers’ incarceration elevates mothers’ stress levels or negatively affects parenting practices, how these effects might be mediated by mothers’ repartnering, and whether these challenges are transmitted to children.

At a time when paternal incarceration is on the rise, this study takes important steps to examine the effects of paternal incarceration on children’s early development. Our findings suggest that when fathers spend time in prison or jail, they place their young children at risk for behavioral problems at the start of school. Boys and children who lived in the same household as their fathers prior to incarceration, in particular, may have difficulty meeting behavioral demands at home and at school. Finding ways to minimize this risk by helping children exposed to paternal incarceration and their caregivers and teachers should be of utmost importance to researchers and policymakers.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the Annie E. Casey Foundation. We thank them for their support but acknowledge that the findings and conclusions presented in this report are those of the authors alone, and do not necessarily reflect the opinions of the Foundation. The Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study was supported by Grant R01HD36916 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), and the project described in this article was supported by Award Number R24HD058486 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver NICHD. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver NICHD or the National Institutes of Health. We are thankful for the suggestions of the Fragile Families Working Group and of the reviewers and Editor of Demography.

Notes

1

Further details on the disposition data are available from the authors upon request.

2

Missing data on individual survey items is modeled using a series of dummy variables.

3

As noted earlier, fathers are not asked about incarceration at year 5, although they are asked about criminal charges and convictions. The father-based report in Table 1 therefore notes any disposition reports of incarceration; indirect reports of incarceration; or, in cases where the fathers report conviction, reports of incarceration by their partners. We consider these reports as part of the fathers’ measure under the assumption that their self-reported conviction suggests willingness to report antisocial activity. These reports therefore reduce any bias created by the error in the survey questionnaire.

References

Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S. H., Weisgias-Kuperus, N., Van Goudoever, J. B., & Oosterlaan, J. (
2009
).
Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children
.
Pediatrics
,
124
,
717
728
. 10.1542/peds.2008-2816
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (
2000
).
Manual for the AESBA preschool forms and profiles
.
Burlington
:
University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families
.
Amato, P. R. (
2006
).
Marital discord, divorce, and children’s well-being: Results from a 20-year longitudinal study of two generations
. In Clarke-Stewart, A., & Dunn, J. F. (Eds.),
Families count: Effects on child and adolescent development
.
New York
:
Cambridge University Press
.
Anderson, E. (
1999
).
Code of the street: Decency, violence, and the moral life of the inner city
.
New York
:
W. W. Norton & Company
.
Arditti, J. A., Lambert-Shute, J., & Joest, K. (
2003
).
Saturday morning at the jail: Implications of incarceration for families and children
.
Family Relations
,
52
,
195
204
. 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2003.00195.x
Arditti, J. A., Smock, S. A., & Parkman, T. S. (
2005
).
“It’s been hard to be a father”: A qualitative exploration of incarcerated fatherhood
.
Fathering
,
3
,
267
288
. 10.3149/fth.0303.267
Argys, L., Peters, E., Cook, S., Garasky, S., Nepomnyaschy, L., & Sorensen, E. (
2006
).
Measuring contact between children and nonresident fathers
. In Hofferth, S., & Casper, L. (Eds.),
Handbook of measurement issues in family research
(pp.
375
398
).
Mahwah, NJ
:
Lawrence Erlbaum
.
Bank, L., Dishion, T., Skinner, M., & Patterson, G. R. (
1990
).
Method variance in structural equation modeling: Living with GLOP
. In Patterson, G. R. (Ed.),
Depression and aggression in family interaction
(pp.
247
279
).
Hillsdale, NJ
:
Erlbaum
.
Bloom, D. (2006). Employment-focused programs for ex-prisoners: What have we learned, what are we learning, and where should we go from here? In Research on prisoner reentry: What do we know and what do we want to know. Ann Arbor: Gerald R. Ford School of Public Policy, University of Michigan.
Bowlby, J. (
1973
).
Separation: Anxiety and anger
.
New York
:
Basic Books
.
Braman, D. S. (
2004
).
Doing time on the outside: Incarceration and family life in urban America
.
Ann Arbor
:
University of Michigan Press
.
Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Smart, M., Jackson, J., Tagami, S., & Moffitt, T. E. (
2001
).
Can women provide reliable information about their children’s fathers? Cross-informant agreement about men’s lifetime antisocial behaviour
.
Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines
,
42
,
915
920
. 10.1111/1469-7610.00787
Comfort, M. (
2008
).
Doing time together: Love and family in the shadow of the prison
.
Chicago, IL
:
University of Chicago Press
.
Crosnoe, R., & Cooper, C. E. (
2010
).
Economically disadvantaged children’s transitions into elementary school: Linking family and school contexts to inform policy
.
American Educational Research Journal
,
47
,
258
291
. 10.3102/0002831209351564
Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., & Fox, H. C. (
2004
).
The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: Following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
86
,
130
147
. 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.130
Dickman, S. J. (
1990
).
Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: Personality and cognitive correlates
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
58
,
95
102
. 10.1037/0022-3514.58.1.95
Edin, K. (
2000
).
Few good men: Why poor mothers don’t marry or remarry
.
American Prospect
,
11
,
26
31
.
Edin, K., Nelson, T. J., & Paranal, R. (
2004
).
Fatherhood and incarceration as potential turning points in the criminal careers of unskilled men
. In Pattillo, M., Weiman, D. F., & Western, B. (Eds.),
Imprisoning America: The social effects of mass incarceration
(pp.
46
75
).
New York
:
Russell Sage Foundation
.
Eiden, R. D., Edwards, E. P., & Leonard, K. E. (
2007
).
A conceptual model for the development of externalizing behavior problems among kindergarten children of alcoholic families: Role of parenting and children’s self-regulation
.
Developmental Psychology
,
43
,
1187
1201
. 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1187
Farrington, D. P. (
1998
).
Predictors, causes, and correlates of male youth violence
.
Crime and Justice
,
24
,
421
475
. 10.1086/449284
Geller, A., Garfinkel, I., Cooper, C. E., & Mincy, R. B. (
2009
).
Parental incarceration and child wellbeing: Implications for urban families
.
Social Science Quarterly
,
90
,
1186
1202
. 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00653.x
Geller, A., Garfinkel, I., & Western, B. (
2011
).
Incarceration and support for children in fragile families
.
Demography
,
48
,
25
47
. 10.1007/s13524-010-0009-9
Glaze, L. E., & Maruschak, L. M. (
2009
).
Parents in prison and their minor children (Bureau of Justice Statistics Special Report)
.
Washington, DC
:
National Institute of Justice
.
Golub, A., Johnson, B. D., Taylor, A., & Liberty, H. J. (
2002
).
The validity of arrestees’ self-reports: Variations across questions and persons
.
Justice Quarterly
,
19
,
477
502
. 10.1080/07418820200095321
Gottfredson, M., & Hirschi, T. (
1990
).
A general theory of crime
.
Stanford, CA
:
Stanford University Press
.
Greene, W. H. (
2003
).
Econometric analysis
.
Upper Saddle River, NJ
:
Prentice Hall
.
Groves, R. M. (
2004
).
Survey errors and survey costs
.
Hoboken, NJ
:
Wiley
.
Hairston, C. F. (
1998
).
The forgotten parent: Understanding the forces that influence incarcerated fathers’ relationships with their children
.
Child Welfare
,
77
,
617
639
.
Harrison, P. M., & Beck, A. J. (
2005
).
Prisoners in 2004 (Bureau of Justice Statistics Bulletin)
.
Washington, DC
:
Bureau of Justice Statistics
.
Jacobs, E., & Western, B. (
2007
).
Report on the evaluation of the ComALERT prisoner reentry program
.
Brooklyn, NY
:
Kings County District Attorney
.
Johnson, E. I., & Waldfogel, J. (
2002
).
Parental incarceration: Recent trends and implications for child welfare
.
Social Service Review
,
76
,
460
479
. 10.1086/341184
Kahn, R. S., Zuckerman, B., Bauchner, H., Homer, C. J., & Wise, P. H. (
2002
).
Women’s health after pregnancy and child outcomes at age 3 years: A prospective cohort study
.
American Journal of Public Health
,
92
,
1312
1318
. 10.2105/AJPH.92.8.1312
Kaushal, N. (
2007
).
Do food stamps cause obesity? Evidence from immigrant experience
.
Journal of Health Economics
,
26
,
968
991
. 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.01.006
Lewis, C. E., Garfinkel, I., & Gao, Q. (
2007
).
Incarceration and unwed fathers in fragile families
.
Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare
,
34
(
3
),
77
94
.
Lindquist, C., & Bir, A. (
2008
).
The national evaluation of the responsible fatherhood, marriage and family strengthening grants for incarcerated and re-entering fathers and their partners: Program overview and evaluation summary (ASPE Research Brief)
.
Washington, DC
:
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
.
Lundberg, S., McLanahan, S., & Rose, E. (
2007
).
Child gender and father involvement in fragile families
.
Demography
,
44
,
79
92
. 10.1353/dem.2007.0007
McHale, S. M., Kim, J-Y, Dotterer, A. M., Crouter, A. C., & Booth, A. (
2009
).
The development of gendered interests and personality qualities from middle childhood through adolescence: A biosocial analysis
.
Child Development
,
80
,
482
495
. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01273.x
McLoyd, V. C. (
1998
).
Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development
.
American Psychologist
,
53
,
185
204
. 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185
Meadows, S. O., McLanahan, S. S., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (
2007
).
Parental depression and anxiety and early childhood behavior problems across family types
.
Journal of Marriage and Family
,
69
,
1162
1177
. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00439.x
Moeller, F. G., Barratt, E. S., Dougherty, D. M., Schmitz, J. M., & Swann, A. C. (
2001
).
Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity
.
American Journal of Psychiatry
,
158
,
1783
1793
. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783
Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (
2005
).
Parental imprisonment: Effects on boys’ antisocial behaviour and delinquency through the life-course
.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
,
46
,
1269
1278
. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01433.x
Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (
2008
).
The effects of parental imprisonment on children
.
Crime and Justice
,
37
,
133
206
.
Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (
2008
).
Parental imprisonment: Long-lasting effects on boys’ internalizing problems through the life course
.
Development and Psychopathology
,
20
,
273
290
. 10.1017/S0954579408000138
Murray, J., Farrington, D. P., Sekol, I., & Olsen, R. F. (2009). Effects of parental imprisonment on child antisocial behaviour and mental health: A systematic review. In Campbell Systematic Reviews. Oslo, Norway: The Campbell Collaboration.
New York Times. (2009, December 25). Smart answers to recidivism. P. A30.
Parke, R., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2002, January 30–31). Effects of parental incarceration on young children. Paper prepared for the conference “From Prison to Home,” U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Washington, DC.
Petersilia, J. (
2003
).
When prisoners come home: Parole and prisoner re-entry
.
New York
:
Oxford University Press
.
Phillips, S. D., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G. P., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (
2006
).
Disentangling the risks: Parent criminal justice involvement and children’s exposure to family risks
.
Criminology and Public Policy
,
5
,
677
702
. 10.1111/j.1745-9133.2006.00404.x
Pianta, R. C., Cox, M. J., & Snow, K. L. (
2007
).
School readiness and the transition to kindergarten in the era of accountability
.
Baltimore, MD
:
Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
.
Reichman, N., Teitler, J., Garfinkel, I., & McLanahan, S. (
2001
).
Fragile families: Sample and design
.
Children and Youth Services Review
,
23
,
303
326
. 10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4
Roy, K. M., & Dyson, O. L. (
2005
).
Gatekeeping in context: Babymama drama and the involvement of incarcerated fathers
.
Fathering
,
3
,
267
288
. 10.3149/fth.0303.289
Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (
1990
).
Crime and deviance over the life course: The salience of adult social bonds
.
American Sociological Review
,
55
,
609
627
. 10.2307/2095859
Schwartz-Soicher, O., Geller, A., & Garfinkel, I. (
2011
).
The effects of paternal incarceration on material hardship
.
Social Service Review
,
85
,
447
473
. 10.1086/661925
Solomon, A. L., & Zweig, J. (2006, November 6). Overview of the issue: Parents in the criminal justice system and its impact on children and families. In Children of Parents in the Criminal Justice System: Children at Risk. Bethesda, MD.
Sroufe, L. A. (
1988
).
The role of infant-caregiver attachment in development
. In Belsky, J., & Nezworski, T. (Eds.),
Clinical implications of attachment
(pp.
18
40
).
Hillsdale, NJ
:
Lawrence Erlbaum
.
Swisher, R. R., & Waller, M. R. (
2008
).
Confining fatherhood: Incarceration and paternal involvement among nonresident white, African American, and Latino fathers
.
Journal of Family Issues
,
29
,
1067
1088
. 10.1177/0192513X08316273
Tach, L., Mincy, R., & Edin, K. (
2010
).
Parenting as a “package deal”: Relationships, fertility, and nonresident father involvement among unmarried parents
.
Demography
,
47
,
181
204
. 10.1353/dem.0.0096
Wakefield, S., & Wildeman, C. (
2011
).
Mass imprisonment and racial disparities in childhood behavioral problems
.
Criminology and Public Policy
,
10
,
793
817
. 10.1111/j.1745-9133.2011.00740.x
Waller, M. R., & Swisher, R. R. (
2006
).
Fathers’ risk factors in fragile families: Implications for “healthy” relationships and father involvement
.
Social Problems
,
53
,
392
420
. 10.1525/sp.2006.53.3.392
Wechsler, D. (
1981
).
Wechsler adult intelligence scale—Revised (WAIS-R Manual)
.
New York
:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, for The Psychological Corporation
.
Western, B. (
2006
).
Punishment and inequality in America
.
New York
:
Russell Sage Foundation
.
Western, B., Kling, J. R., & Weiman, D. F. (
2001
).
The labor market consequences of incarceration
.
Crime and Delinquency
,
47
,
410
427
. 10.1177/0011128701047003007
Western, B., & Wildeman, C. (
2009
).
The black family and mass incarceration
.
The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science
,
621
,
221
242
. 10.1177/0002716208324850
Whitaker, R. C., Orzol, S. M., & Kahn, R. S. (
2006
).
Maternal mental health, substance use, and domestic violence in the year after delivery and subsequent behavior problems in children at age 3 years
.
Archives of General Psychiatry
,
63
,
551
560
. 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.551
Wilbur, M. B., Marani, J. E., Appugliese, D., Woods, R., Siegel, J. A., Cabral, H. J., & Frank, D. A. (
2007
).
Socioemotional effects of fathers’ incarceration on low-income, urban, school-aged children
.
Pediatrics
,
120
,
e678
e685
. 10.1542/peds.2006-2166
Wildeman, C. (
2010
).
Paternal incarceration and children’s physically aggressive behaviors: Evidence from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study
.
Social Forces
,
89
,
285
309
. 10.1353/sof.2010.0055
Wu, L. L., & Wolfe, B. L. (
2001
).
Out of wedlock: Causes and consequences of nonmarital fertility
.
New York
:
Russell Sage Foundation
.